Risk Taking in Hospitalized Patients with Acute and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury ´ ´ Shirley Fecteau1,2*, Jean Levasseur-Moreau1, Alberto Garcıa-Molina3, Hatiche Kumru3, Raul ´ Pelayo Vergara3, Monste Bernabeu3, Teresa Roig3, Alvaro Pascual-Leone2,3, Jose Maria Tormos3 ´adaptation et Inte ´gration Sociale, Centre de Recherche Universitaire en Sante Mentale de Quebec, Medical School, Laval ´ 1 Centre Interdisciplinaire de Recherche en Re University, Quebec city, Quebec, Canada, 2 Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, ´ Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, 3 Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitacio adscrit a la UAB, Badalona, Barcelona, Spain; Universitat ` `s), ´ ´ `ncies de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol, Badalona, Barcelona, Spain Autonoma de Barcelona, Bellaterra (Cerdanyola del Valle Spain; Fundacio Institut d’Investigacio en Cie Abstract Rehabilitation can improve cognitive deficits observed in patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI). However, despite rehabilitation, the ability of making a choice often remains impaired. Risk taking is a daily activity involving numerous cognitive processes subserved by a complex neural network. In this work we investigated risk taking using the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) in patients with acute TBI and healthy controls. We hypothesized that individuals with TBI will take less risk at the BART as compared to healthy individuals. We also predicted that within the TBI group factors such as the number of days since the injury, severity of the injury, and sites of the lesion will play a role in risk taking as assessed with the BART. Main findings revealed that participants with TBI displayed abnormally cautious risk taking at the BART as compared to healthy subjects. Moreover, healthy individuals showed increased risk taking throughout the task which is in line with previous work. However, individuals with TBI did not show this increased risk taking during the task. We also investigated the influence of three patients’ characteristics on their performance at the BART: Number of days post injury, Severity of the head injury, and Status of the frontal lobe. Results indicate that performance at the BART was influenced by the number of days post injury and the status of the frontal lobe, but not by the severity of the head injury. Reported findings are encouraging for risk taking seems to naturally improve with time postinjury. They support the need of conducting longitudinal prospective studies to ultimately identify impaired and intact cognitive skills that should be trained postinjury. ´ Citation: Fecteau S, Levasseur-Moreau J, Garcıa-Molina A, Kumru H, Vergara RP, et al. (2013) Risk Taking in Hospitalized Patients with Acute and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. PLoS ONE 8(12): e83598. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598 Editor: Anthony E. Kline, University of Pittsburgh, United States of America Received June 21, 2013; Accepted November 5, 2013; Published December 26, 2013 Copyright: ß 2013 Fecteau et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This work was supported by grants from the Canada Research Chair in Cognitive Neuroplasticity to SF, from the National Center for Research Resources: Harvard-Thorndike General Clinical Research Center at BIDMC (NCRR MO1 RR01032) and Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (UL1 ´ RR025758), NIH grant K24 RR018875 to APL, and by the Foundation La Marato TV3 (071931) and grant PI082004 from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III to JMT. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. * E-mail: shirley.fecteau@fmed.ulaval.ca Introduction Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is an important cause of disability among adults, resulting in tremendous human and financial cost [1,2]. A TBI is caused by external forces, such as a blow, jolt or penetration to the head temporarily or permanently disrupting brain functions [3]. Thanks to advanced medical care, the survival rate of patients with TBI continues to increase [4–6]. However, patients can experience lifelong impairments across virtually all domains of functioning, including physical, cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and social areas [7]. Cognitive and behavioral deficits seem to be especially deleterious impacting overall functional outcomes and quality of life of patients and their families [8–15]. The extent of these deficits following TBI is broad and includes information processing [16], attention [17], memory [18], executive function [19], computation, and discrimination of probabilities [20]. They can occur singly or in combination, can change in severity over time, and often remain greatly impaired despite rehabilitation [10,21–23]. These processes need to be integrated when assessing the risk to take or not relative to making a choice and, indeed, often remains impaired long after the injury PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 and can critically hinders social autonomy [24]. It thus seems important to carefully identify impaired and intact processes involved in risk taking and characterize factors that may be associated with them (e.g., time course, severity of the injury). Such further knowledge may contribute to eventually develop effective and individualized rehabilitation program to improve risk taking in individuals with TBI. We adopt here the definition of risk taking proposed by Leigh [25], that is ‘‘behaviors that involve some potential for danger or harm while also providing an opportunity to obtain some form of reward’’. In the present study we investigated risk taking behaviors in TBI patients using the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Our general hypothesis was that subjects with TBI will display risk aversive response style at the BART as compared to healthy controls. This prediction was based on previous work [26–28] reporting impaired risk taking and decision-making in TBI patients compared to controls, using however more complex tasks (e.g., the Iowa Gambling Task) than the BART task. The BART is a behavioral measure of risk taking with real-world convergence. Specifically, it has been correlated with occurrence of real-world risk behaviors [29–31], such as substance use [32], risky sexual December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury behaviors [33], and delinquent behaviors [29–31,34], as well as self-report measures of risk-related constructs such as sensation seeking, impulsivity, and deficiencies in behavioral constraints [29]. In the context of the present study involving patients with TBI, the BART has the advantage of not involving working memory and calculation per se. This is particularly important when studying performance of patients with acute TBI as memory and attention are often impaired [35] and could be confound factors in the study of risk taking. Obviously in real world situations, numerous cognitive processes are involved when taking risk [36,37]. However, the literature on risk taking behaviors in acute TBI patients is still scarce and there is a need to gather a fine characterization of specific deficits contributing to impaired risk taking behaviors in this population. Here we chose to focus on aspects of risk taking that do not involve memory or calculation. Aspects of risk taking studied at the BART involve choices made in the context of increasing risk in ambiguous situations [29]. Ambiguity can be viewed as any situation in which the likelihood of one or more of the payoffs occurring is not fully specified [38,39]. The BART meets this definition of an ambiguous decision in that one of the choices (to make another balloon pump) has unknown probabilities. At each decision point, the participant has to make a choice between a chance for an incremental gain or potentially larger losses with unknown probabilities versus a 100% certainty of no loss but no additional gain. We also investigated whether performance at the BART in individuals with TBI differed according to the Number of days post injury, the Severity of the head injury, and the Frontal lobe status (described below). Table 1. Characteristics of the subjects. Subjects P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 P10 P11 P12 P13 P14 P15 P16 P17 P18 P19 P20 Gender M M M M F M F F M M M M M M M M F M M M M M M M F M M M M M F M M M M M M F Age 27 35 28 29 27 25 58 26 36 23 24 31 24 24 36 57 30 38 19 37 32 29 25 34 16 31 26 38 50 22 29 29 20 28 19 33 37 31 N of days since the injury 44 44 45 45 52 54 55 68 71 87 88 92 97 99 103 104 107 133 154 155 156 170 171 176 193 211 241 282 286 288 - Glasgow score 9 3 0 5 7 5 13 5 13 11 6 0 6 9 3 15 9 0 5 3 7 12 4 5 4 4 4 3 0 9 - Methods Participants Individuals with acute TBI and healthy adults took part of the study. The TBI group was composed of 30 in-patient subjects at the Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitacio Guttmann-UAB (5 ´ women) with an average age of 31 years old (range of 16 to 58 years). Severity of their initial injury was diagnosed using the Glasgow coma scale (GCS) with mild (N = 3; GCS $ 13), moderate (N = 6; GCS 9–12) or severe TBI (N = 21; GCS # 13). The average number of days between their injury and their participation at this experiment was 129 days (range of 44 to 288 days). The healthy group consisted of 8 adults (2 women) with an average age of 28 years old (range of 19 to 37 years). Exclusion criteria for the healthy subjects were aged under 18 years old and history of any neurological or psychiatric disorders. Participants or their legal guardian gave informed written consent prior to entering the study, which was approved by the Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitacio Guttmann-UAB ethics committee (Barce´ lona, Spain). Please refer to Table 1 for details. P21 P22 P23 P24 P25 P26 P27 P28 P29 P30 H1 H2 H3 H4 H5 H6 H7 H8 Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) In the BART, participants have to make a choice in a context of increasing risk. They are invited to inflate a computerized balloon by pushing a ‘pump’ (see Figure 1). The balloon can explode at any moment. Participants have to decide after each pump whether to keep pumping and risk explosion of the balloon, or to stop. Participants accumulate money in a temporary bank with each pump (5 cents for each pump). When the participant decides to stop pumping, the accumulated money is transferred to a permanent bank. However, if the balloon explodes, all of the money accumulated in the temporary bank is lost. Therefore, the probability of losing the money, as well as the potential lost (i.e., the amount of money) increases with each pump. Each balloon has PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.t001 a different explosion point. There are a total of thirty trials (balloons). The BART was conducted in an experimental room equipped with a PC computer. Instructions for the BART task were written so that all participants received the same instructions, and participants were invited to ask any question they may have after reading the instructions. They were given no precise information about the probability of explosion or the total amount of money acquired from previous participants. Participants were told that the subject with the highest amount of money would receive a gift certificate. December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury and those with spared frontal lobe (N = 16) performed differently at the risk taking task. We hypothesized that within the TBI group, patients with frontal lesions in the right hemisphere will take more risk than patients with no frontal lesion. Data analysis The main outcome measure was the averaged adjusted number of pumps (i.e., number of pumps for balloons that did not explode), which has been advocated as the preferred dependent measure for the BART because it avoids the constraints on individual differences that occur on trials with explosions (for which there is a fixed limit of potential pumps; [29,34]). We also calculated the time course of performance on the adjusted number of pumps (i.e., 10 first balloons; 10 second balloons; 10 last balloons) because there is typically an increase of risk taking as the task goes (i.e., increased number of pumps). We also calculated the total of money earned (although this measure is inherently linked to the number of pumps) because the goal pursued by the subjects in this task is to earn as much money as possible. Finally, we calculated the averaged number of explosions. When testing for differences between the TBI and healthy groups, we used analysis of variance (ANOVA). We subsequently assessed homogeneity using the Mauchly’s test and we applied the Greenhouse-Geisser correction when appropriate. Results with a p value # 0.05 were considered significant for all statistical analyses. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 21.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago). There was no difference between the TBI and control groups for the age (t (36) = 0.812; p = 0.422) or gender (X2 (1) = 0.292; p = 0.589). Figure 1. Schematic representation of the BART experiment. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.g001 Patients’ characteristics In order to further explore risk taking behaviors in TBI patients, we tested whether performance at the BART was related to any one of the three following patients’ characteristics. 1. Number of days post injury. We tested whether the number of days between patients’ injury and participation at the BART experiment was related to the BART performance. Although the general ability of risk taking seems to remain impaired in TBI patients, some cognitive functions involved in risk taking, such as attention and information processing, may improve at specific periods post-injury with usual rehabilitation therapy [21,22,40]. It thus seems important to identify whether risk taking remains impaired regardless of the number of days since the injury or whether it indicates a natural recovery or improvement. Based on studies suggesting cognitive improvement with functions associated with risk taking [21,22], we hypothesized that within the TBI group, performance at the BART will be impaired (that is they will be risk averse), especially for patients with the smallest number of days since injury. 2. Severity of the head injury. The second patients’ characteristic we explored was the severity of the head injury. This was measured with the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS). The GCS is one of the most common methods to characterize severity of head injuries [41]. This scale relies on observation of orientation, movement and verbal abilities of the patient and yields from mild, moderate to severe classification on a continuous scale. We used GCS scores to test whether severity of head injuries were related to the performance at the studied risk taking task in our TBI patients. We hypothesized that within the TBI group, patients with more severe head injury (i.e., lower GCS score) will display lower performance at the BART (i.e., risk averse behaviors). 3. Frontal lobe status. The third patients’ characteristic we studied was the status of their frontal lobe, whether a lesion was reported or not on their medical records based on their CT or MRI scans. Of note, analyses of CT and MRI scans (e.g., lesion size) and inter-rater reliability tests were not performed. Lesion work and neuroimaging studies in healthy subjects have repeatedly associated risk taking and frontal lobes [36,42–44]. Adults with ventromedial/orbitofrontal lesions appear to engage in immediate, higher rewards and seem insensitive to potential future losses [45]. There is still no clear consensus in regards to the frontal hemispheric contribution on riskier behaviors and impaired decision-making skills, but greater deficits have been found with lesion in the right frontal lobe [43,44]. Based on this literature, we thus tested whether patients with damaged frontal lobe (N = 14) PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 Results Performance at the BART in TBI patients and healthy controls Examination of the BART data indicates a group difference on the average number of pumps on balloons that did not explode (AVOVA; F(1,36) = 13.024; p = 0.001). As shown in Figure 2, the average number of pumps was smaller for the TBI group than the healthy group (20 and 40.4 pumps, respectively). For the time course at the BART (i.e., the average number of pumps on balloons that did not explode for the first 10 balloons, the second 10 balloons, and the last 10 balloons), there was an Figure 2. Graphic display of the average number of adjusted pumps (the total pumps of the balloons that did not explode) for each group. Error bars indicate SEM. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.g002 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury effect of group (repeated measures ANOVA with group and time as variables; F(1,36) = 13.804; p = 0.001), an effect of time (F(1.4,48.9) = 11.119; p = 0.001), and a significant interaction group by time (F(1.4,48.9) = 4.641; p = 0.026). Post-hoc analysis (using Sidak-Holm correction) revealed that groups significantly differed ` when comparing the first and second balloons (p = 0.038), the second and last balloons (p = 0.020) and when comparing the first and last balloons (p = 0.006). Control participants showed a substantial increase in risk taking with time. As illustrated in Figure 3, healthy controls pumped more toward the end as compared as the beginning of the experiment, whereas participants with TBI did not show a significant increase with time. We also ran the full model with a repeated measures ANOVA with these three groups (i.e., TBI with right hemisphere lesion, TBI with spared right hemisphere, and healthy controls) and time. We found an effect of group (AVOVA; F(2,35) = 10.499; p = 0.0001), an effect of time F(1.4,49.3) = 11.099; p = 0.0001), and an interaction of group by time F(2.8,49.3) = 4.811; p = 0.006). Posthoc analysis (Sidak-Holm method) revealed a group difference ` between the TBI subjects with no right hemispheric lesion and the healthy subjects (p = 0.0001). These two groups also performed differently between the first and the second sets of 10 balloons (p = 0.006) and between the second and third sets of 10 balloons (p = 0.026). However, these groups did not significantly differed between the second and the third sets of 10 second balloons (p = 0.051). There was also a group difference for the total amount of money earned (ANOVA; F(1,37) = 10.541; p = 0.003). The TBI group earned a smaller amount of money than the healthy group ($22 and $39 respectively). There was also a group difference on the averaged number of explosions (ANOVA; F(1,36) = 18.246; p,0.0001). The averaged numbers of explosions were 4.2 and 10.5 in the TBI group and healthy group, respectively. Performance at the BART and patients’ characteristics 1. Number of days post injury. The correlation between the main BART outcome and the number of days post injury (i.e., between the injury and the BART performance) was not significant (Pearson correlation; r = 0.330; 2-tailed value: p = 0.075). For the time course, the number of days post injury did not significantly correlate with performance in the first 10 balloons (r = 0.264; 2-tailed value: p = 0.158), the second 10 balloons (r = 0.335; 2-tailed value: p = 0.070), or the last 10 balloons (r = 0.333; 2-tailed value: p = 0.072). There was however a significant positive correlation between the number of days post injury and the total of money earned (r = 0.366; 2-tailed value: p = 0.047). This is illustrated in Figure 4. 2. Severity of the head injury. There was no significant correlation between the main BART outcome measure and GCS data (Pearson correlation; r = 0.111; 2-tailed value: p = 0.559). The GCS data was not correlated with the number of pumps on the first 10 balloons (r = 0.109; 2-tailed value: p = 0.565), the second 10 balloons (r = 0.184; 2-tailed value: p = 0.330), or the last 10 balloons (r = 0.240; 2-tailed value: p = 0.202). There was no correlation between the amount of money earned and the GCS data (r = 0.116; 2-tailed value: p = 0.542). 3. Frontal lobe status. There was no group difference when comparing patients with a frontal lesion in the right hemisphere (N = 14) to those with no frontal lesion in the right hemisphere (N = 16) on the main BART outcome measure (the average adjusted number of pumps; ANOVA; F(1,28) = 2.563; p = 0.121). There was no group difference for the first 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 0.770; p = 0.388). However there were significant group differences for the second 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 4.568; p = 0.041) and the last 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 6.897; p = 0.014). This is illustrated in Figure 5. There was no group difference for the total amount of money earned (F(1,28) = 2.905; p = 0.099). We also examined for a possible link between performance at the BART and the presence or absence of a lesion in the left frontal lobe. None of the measures reached significance (ANOVA; main BART outcome (F(1,28) = 0.012; p = 0.915); first 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 0.003; p = 0.954); second 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 0.010; p = 0.920); last 10 balloons (F(1,28) = 0.049; p = 0.826); money earned (F(1,28) = 0.033; p = 0.857). Figure 3. Graphic display of the average number of adjusted pumps for each group and time period (the first set of 10 balloons, the second set of 10 balloons, and the last set of 10 balloons). Black line represents the healthy group and the grey line represents the TBI group. Error bars indicate SEM. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.g003 Figure 4. Graphic display of the mean amount of money for the TBI group according to the number of days since their injury. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.g004 PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury Figure 5. Graphic display of the average number of adjusted pumps for TBI patients according to whether or not they display a frontal lesion in the right hemisphere and time period (the first set of 10 balloons, the second set of 10 balloons, and the last set of 10 balloons). Black line represents patients with no frontal lesion in the right hemisphere and the grey line represents patients with frontal lesion in the right hemisphere. Error bars indicate SEM. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083598.g005 Discussion In the present work we investigated risk taking in patients with acute TBI and healthy controls. The main findings revealed that TBI patients displayed impaired risk taking. Indeed, they showed abnormally risk averse behaviors at the BART paradigm as compared to healthy controls. Moreover, healthy controls displayed an increasing risk taking throughout the task. This sort of learning curve throughout pumping the thirty balloons has been reported in previous studies [31,32]. Subjects take more risk with the last set of ten balloons as compared to the second and first sets of ten balloons. This increasing risk was not found in the TBI group. Patients barely increased their risk taking throughout the task. This result differs from that of Chiu and colleagues [46]. These authors investigated the neural correlates of risk taking at the BART in TBI patients using functional magnetic resonance imaging. TBI patients and healthy subjects differed in terms of patterns of brain activations when performing the BART. However, behavioural responses at the BART (i.e., risk taking) was not significantly different between the two groups. As the authors mentioned, this negative behavioural finding may be explained by their small sample size (11 TBI patients and 8 healthy subjects). We further investigated risk taking at the BART in TBI patients according to three characteristics. These variables were chosen in order to identify whether characteristics of the injury influenced risk taking. These were the Number of days post injury, Severity of the head injury, and Frontal lobe status. Effects of number of days post injury on risk taking in TBI patients. There was a correlation between the number of days post injury and the amount of money earned at the BART. The smallest number of days post injury was related with the smallest amount of money earned. This effect was modest, but it suggests a natural improvement in risk taking soon after the injury, within the period of 44 to 288 days post-injury. This is in line with natural improvement of cognitive performance (e.g., executive PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 functions, memory, processing speed, language abilities, constructional skills) that has been previously observed during the first year postinjury [47], with greater improvement during the first 5-6 months [48]. Of note, although risk taking seems to improve with time, it remained greatly impaired as compared to healthy subjects. Effects of severity of the head injury on risk taking in TBI patients. Previous work indicates that cognitive deficits such as executive dysfunction are related to severity of TBI [47,49,50]. This was not observed here. Of note, our sample size was limited (N = 30) and the number of patients according to the severity of the TBI was unequal (three patients with mild TBI, six with moderate TBI, and twenty-one with severe TBI). This may explain the lack of group difference based on severity of the injury. Also, group difference based on the severity of the injury on cognitive skills has mainly been reported in chronic patients [47,49–51]. It is possible that during the acute phase (as tested here), severity of the head injury does not consist of a critical factor on risk taking. Here patients regardless of the severity of their injury displayed very poor performance at the BART, pumping twice as less the balloons than healthy subjects (20 vs. 40 pumps). Furthermore, definition of the severity of head injury solely based on GCS scores (as in this study) may be insufficient. As Novack and colleagues [47] suggested ‘‘Defining the severity of injury based on a combination of factors (such as GCS score, duration of PTA, and signs of structural lesion on neuroimaging) is a commendable research goal’’. Effects of frontal lobe status on risk taking in TBI patients. Cicerone and colleagues [19] have proposed that the time course of improvement of cognitive functions postinjury in TBI varies depending on severity of focal and diffuse effects. Here, patients with a lesion in the frontal lobe of the right hemisphere (N = 14) and patients with no lesion in the frontal lobe of the right hemisphere (N = 16) performed differently during the second and third sets of balloons at the BART, but they did not differ during the first set of balloons. More specifically, patients with a lesion in the frontal lobe of the right hemisphere showed an increase risk taking as compared to patients with no lesion in the right frontal hemisphere, although they all started with a similar performance (i.e., for the first set). This was not observed in regards to the left hemisphere (18 patients with a lesion in the left frontal lobe and 12 patients with no lesion in the left frontal lobe). Moreover, we observed that performance of TBI patients with right hemisphere lesion differed even more from healthy controls than TBI patients with no right hemisphere lesion. As discussed above, healthy subjects usually show a learning curve throughout completion of the BART [30,32]. Previous work reported that patients with frontal lesions display impaired risk taking and decision-making by engaging in more immediate, higher rewards (they bet more) than healthy subjects [52,53]. There is however no clear consensus in regards to the hemispheric contribution on risk taking impairments in patients with frontal lesion. Some suggested that impaired risk taking and decision-making were linked to ventromedial lesions in the right hemisphere [43] – and our results support this – , but others proposed they were linked to ventromedial/ orbitofrontal lesions regardless of the hemisphere [54,55] or to the total frontal size and right nonventral frontal lesions [36,44]. Study Limitations Several limitations should be considered when interpreting the present results. First, as discussed above, the sample size was small. Also, although the number of days between patient’s participation to the study and their injury was informative, that is indicating a curve of improved risk taking behaviors, a better design would December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury have been to assess the same patients at different time points. We believe it was fortunate to evaluate TBI patients in their acute phase. However, we could assess them only once while they were inpatient and unfortunately it was not possible to follow them once they completed their inpatient care. More information on patients would also be important in future work to better understand risk taking in TBI. It would be important to further investigate the potential influence of lesion characteristics (e.g., size, precise location) using patient’s MRI on cognitive performance. In this study, we had access to the medical records that included the neurological report but not the scans. It would also be interesting to test the impact on risk taking in patients with concurrent conditions such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as ADHD has been associated with impaired risk taking [56]. Finally, inclusion of other tasks would also be informative to further interpretation of our findings. For instance, it would be interesting to assess functions known to be often impaired in TBI patients such as response speed [57], response inhibition [58], and attention [59]. These functions might have played a role in our results. (e.g., Iowa gambling task), patients here were severely impaired as compared to healthy controls (i.e., pumping twice as less the balloons). However, various processes still need to be integrated in order to earn money at this task. It is a goal-directed task involving reward seeking (i.e., to earn money) which demands initiating purposeful behavior (i.e., to pump or stop pumping the balloon) and anticipating consequences of their behavior (i.e., the more they pump, the greater money they earn, but so are the chances that the balloon explodes and then lose the money). It would be interesting to decipher which of these processes are impaired or intact in future work. As pinpointed by Donovan [60], assessment of cognitive functions following TBI is important for guiding rehabilitation. There is still no consensus on the optimal time window during which rehabilitation is more effective [61]: it has been suggested that it is best to intervene earlier [14] or later postinjury [62]. Longitudinal prospective studies assessing multiple cognitive skills should further elucidate potential relations between cognitive deficits and their natural improvement, as well as the impact of rehabilitation on these skills, to ultimately better guide programs. Conclusion In sum, this work shows that risk taking were impaired in patients with TBI during the acute phase. Patients displayed risk averse responses, as compared to healthy controls, to the simple BART task which has real world convergence [29–34]. Although risk taking seemed to improve naturally with time during the acute phase, it remained severely impaired. Also, within our TBI group, patients with frontal lesions in the right hemisphere took more risk at the BART than those with no right frontal lesion. Although the BART experiment is rather simple compared to most cognitive tasks on risk taking that have been used in TBI Acknowledgments We thank all participants and their families for participating to this research project. We would like to thank Jean Leblond for his statistical advice. Author Contributions Conceived and designed the experiments: SF APL JMT. Performed the experiments: SF AGM HK RPV. Analyzed the data: SF JLM AGM HK. Wrote the paper: SF JLM AGM HK RPV MB TR APL JMT. References 1. Gentleman D (2001) Rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury. Trauma 3 193– 204. 2. Shore AD, McCarthy ML, Serpi T, Gertner M (2005) Validity of administrative data for characterizing traumatic brain injury-related hospitalizations. Brain Inj 19, 613–621. 3. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-V Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. 4. Baguley I, Slewa-Younan S, Lazarus R Green A (2000) Long-term mortality trends in patients with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 14, 505–512. 5. Lu J, Marmarou A, Choi S, Maas A, Murray G, et al. (2005) Mortality from traumatic brain injury. Acta Neurochir Suppl 95, 281–285. 6. Stiefel MF, Spiotta A, Gracias VH, Garuffe AM, Guillamondegui O, MaloneyWilensky E, et al. (2005) Reduced mortality rate in patients with severe traumatic brain injury treated with brain tissue oxygen monitoring. J Neurosurg 103, 805–811. 7. Kersel DA, Marsh NV, Havill JH, Sleigh JW (2001) Neuropsychological functioning during the year following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 15, 283–296. 8. Mazaux JM, Masson F, Levin HS, Alaoui P, Maurette P, et al. (1997) Long-term neuropsychological outcome and loss of social autonomy after traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 78, 1316–1320. 9. Nybo T, Koskiniemi M (1999) Cognitive indicators of vocational outcome after severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) in childhood. Brain Inj 13, 759–766. 10. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, Langenbahn DM, Malec JF, et al. (2000) Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 81, 1596–1615. 11. Arciniegas DB, Held K, Wagner P (2002) Cognitive Impairment Following Traumatic Brain Injury. Curr Treat Options Neurol 4, 43–57. 12. Sherer M, Sander AM, Nick TG, High WM Jr, Malec JF, et al. (2002) Early cognitive status and productivity outcome after traumatic brain injury: findings from the TBI model systems. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 83, 183–192. 13. Silver JM, Yudofsky SC, Anderson KE (2005) "Aggressive disorders," in Textbook of Traumatic Brain Injury. 2nd ed., eds. Silver JM Mcallister TW, Yudofsky SC. (Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing), 259–277. 14. Tsaousides T, Gordon WA (2009) Cognitive rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury: assessment to treatment. Mt Sinai J Med 76, 173–181. 15. Cernich AN, Kurtz SM, Mordecai KL, Ryan PB (2010) Cognitive rehabilitation in traumatic brain injury. Curr Treat Options Neurol 12, 412–423. 16. Mathias JL, Beall JA, Bigler ED (2004) Neuropsychological and information processing deficits following mild traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 10, 286–297. 17. Whyte J, Hart T, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, Risser A, et al. (2004) Effects of methylphenidate on attention deficits after traumatic brain injury: a multidimensional, randomized, controlled trial. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 83, 401–420. 18. Mcdowell S, Whyte J, D’esposito M (1997) Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia 35, 1341–1353. 19. Cicerone K, Levin H, Malec J, Stuss D, Whyte J (2006) Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. J Cogn Neurosci 18, 1212–1222. 20. Bonatti E, Zamarian L, Wagner M, Benke T, Hollosi P, et al. (2008) Making decisions and advising decisions in traumatic brain injury. Cogn Behav Neurol 21, 164–175. 21. Park NW, Ingles JL (2001) Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after an acquired brain injury: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 15, 199–210. 22. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, Langenbahn DM, Felicetti T, et al. (2005) Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 86, 1681–1692. 23. Dams-O’connor K, Gordon WA (2010) Role and impact of cognitive rehabilitation. Psychiatr Clin North Am 33, 893–904. 24. Floden D, Alexander MP, Kubu CS, Katz D, Stuss DT (2008) Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 46, 213–223. 25. Leigh BC (1999) Peril, chance, and adventure: Concepts of risk, alcohol use and risky behavior in young adults. Addiction, 94, 371–383. 26. Schlund MW (2002) Effects of acquired brain injury on adaptive choice and the role of reduced sensitivity to contingencies. Brain Inj 16, 527–535. 27. Garcia-Molina A, Roig-Rovira T, Ensenat-Cantallops A, Sanchez-Carrion R, Pico-Azanza N, et al. (2007) [Examination of decision-making processes in patients with traumatic brain injury]. Neurologia 22, 206–212. 28. Sigurdardottir S, Jerstad T, Andelic N, Roe C, Schanke AK (2010) Olfactory dysfunction, gambling task performance and intracranial lesions after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 24, 504–513. 29. Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, et al. (2002) Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl 8, 75–84. PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598 Risk Taking in Acute Traumatic Brain Injury 30. Lejuez CW, Aklin WM, Jones HA, Richards JB, Strong DR, et al. (2003) The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 11, 26–33. 31. Lejuez CW, Aklin WM, Zvolensky MJ, Pedulla CM (2003) Evaluation of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risktaking behaviours. J Adolesc 26, 475–479. 32. Lejuez CW, Aklin W, Bornovalova M, Moolchan ET (2005) Differences in risktaking propensity across inner-city adolescent ever- and never-smokers. Nicotine Tob Res 7, 71–79. 33. Lejuez CW, Simmons BL, Aklin WM, Daughters SB, Dvir S (2004) Risk-taking propensity and risky sexual behavior of individuals in residential substance use treatment. Addict Behav 29, 1643–1647. 34. Aklin WM, Lejuez, CW Zvolensky MJ, Kahler CW, Gwadz M (2005) Evaluation of behavioral measures of risk taking propensity with inner city adolescents. Behav Res Ther 43, 215–228. 35. Arciniegas D, Adler L, Topkoff J, Cawthra E, Filley CM, et al. (1999) Attention and memory dysfunction after traumatic brain injury: cholinergic mechanisms, sensory gating, and a hypothesis for further investigation. Brain Inj 13, 1–13. 36. Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, et al. (2002) Decisionmaking processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain 125, 624–639. 37. Boyle PA, Yu L, Buchman AS, Laibson DI, Bennett DA (2011) Cognitive function is associated with risk aversion in community-based older persons. BMC Geriatr 11, 53. 38. Smith K, Dickhaut J, Mccabe K, Pardo JV (2002) Neuronal substrates for choice under ambiguity, risk, gains, and losses. Management Science 48, 711–718. 39. Zak PJ (2004) Neuroeconomics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 359, 1737– 1748. 40. Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, Sherer M, Nick TG, et al. (2001) Longterm neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 16, 343–355. 41. Teasdale G, Jennett B (1974) Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet 2, 81–84. 42. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 6, 215–225. 43. Tranel D, Bechara A, Denburg NL (2002) Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex 38, 589–612. 44. Clark L, Manes F, Antoun N, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2003) The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 41, 1474–1483. 45. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR (2000) Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10, 295–307. 46. Chiu CY, Tlustos SJ, Walz NC, Holland SK, Eliassen JC, et al. (2012) Neural correlates of risky decision making in adolescents with and without traumatic brain injury using the balloon analog risk task. Dev Neuropsychol 37, 176–183. 47. Novack TA, Alderson AL, Bush BA, Meythaler JM, Canupp K (2000) Cognitive and functional recovery at 6 and 12 months post-TBI. Brain Inj 14, 987–996. 48. Christensen BK, Colella B, Inness E, Hebert D, Monette G, et al. (2008) Recovery of cognitive function after traumatic brain injury: a multilevel modeling analysis of Canadian outcomes. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 89, 3–15. 49. Matheson L (2010) Executive dysfunction, severity of traumatic brain injury, and IQ in workers with disabilities. Work 36, 413–422. 50. Krishnan M, Smith N, Donders J (2012) Use of the Tower of London - Drexel University, Second Edition (TOLDX) in adults with traumatic brain injury. Clin Neuropsychol 26, 951–964. 51. Goldenberg G, Oder W, Spatt J, Podreka I (1992) Cerebral correlates of disturbed executive function and memory in survivors of severe closed head injury: a SPECT study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55, 362–368. 52. Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson SW, et al. (2001) Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia 39, 376–389. 53. Rahman S, J Sahakia B, N Cardinal R, Rogers R, Robbins T (2001) Decision making and neuropsychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci 5, 271–277. 54. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. 55. Bechara A (2004) The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn 55, 30–40. 56. Drechsler R, Rizzo P, Steinhausen HC (2008) Decision-making on an explicit risk-taking task in preadolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Neural Transm 115, 201–209. 57. Stuss DT, Stethem LL, Hugenholtz H, Picton T, Pivik J, et al. (1989) Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 52, 742–748. 58. Leblanc N, Chen S, Swank PR, Ewing-Cobbs L, Barnes M, et al. (2005) Response inhibition after traumatic brain injury (TBI) in children: impairment and recovery. Dev Neuropsychol 28, 829–848. 59. Robertson IH, Manly T, Andrade J, Baddeley BT, Yiend J (1997) ’Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35, 747–758. 60. Donovan NJ, Heaton SC, Kimberg CI, Wen PS, Waid-Ebbs JK, et al. (2011) Conceptualizing functional cognition in traumatic brain injury rehabilitation. Brain Inj 25, 348–364. 61. Gordon WA, Hibbard M (2005) "Cognitive rehabilitation," in Textbook of traumatic brain injury, eds. Silver JM Mcallister TW, Yudofsky SC. (Washington, DC.: American Psychiatric Publishing), 655–660. 62. Kennedy MR, Turkstra L (2006) Group intervention studies in the cognitive rehabilitation of individuals with traumatic brain injury: challenges faced by researchers. Neuropsychol Rev 16, 151–159. PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 December 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 12 | e83598