Certainty of genuine treatment increases drug responses among intellectually disabled patients Karin B. Jensen, PhD Irving Kirsch, PhD Moa Pontén, MS Annelie Rosén, PhD Kathy Yang, MS Randy L. Gollub, MD, PhD Vincent des Portes, MD, PhD Ted J. Kaptchuk, MD Aurore Curie, MD, PhD Correspondence to Dr. Jensen: Karin.Jensen@ki.se ABSTRACT Objective: To determine the placebo component of treatment responses in patients with intellectual disability (ID). Methods: A statistical meta-analysis comparing bias-corrected effect sizes (Hedges g) of drug responses in open-label vs placebo-controlled clinical trials was performed, as these trial types represent different certainty of receiving genuine treatment (100% vs 50%). Studies in fragile X, Down, Prader-Willi, and Williams syndrome published before June 2015 were considered. Results: Seventeen open-label trials (n 5 261, 65% male; mean age 23.6 years; mean trial duration 38 weeks) and 22 placebo-controlled trials (n 5 721, 62% male; mean age 17.1 years; mean trial duration 35 weeks) were included. The overall effect size from pre to post treatment in open-label studies was g 5 0.602 (p 5 0.001). The effect of trial type was statistically significant (p 5 0.001), and revealed higher effect sizes in studies with 100% likelihood of getting active drug, compared to both the drug and placebo arm of placebo-controlled trials. We thus provide evidence for genuine placebo effects, not explainable by natural history or regression toward the mean, among patients with ID. Conclusions: Our data suggest that clinical trials in patients with severe cognitive deficits are influenced by the certainty of receiving genuine medication, and open-label design should thus not be used to evaluate the effect of pharmacologic treatments in ID, as the results will be biased by an enhanced placebo component. Neurology® 2017;88:1912–1918 Supplemental data at Neurology.org 1912 GLOSSARY DSM-V 5 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition; ID 5 intellectual disability. In any pharmacologic treatment, contextual and cognitive factors contribute to the therapeutic response, such as patients’ treatment expectations and knowledge about the treatment.1 These factors, referred to as nonspecific treatment mechanisms, are active ingredients in placebo responses.2 To date, little is known about the contribution of nonspecific treatment effects in trials for patients with severe cognitive deficits. Intellectual disability (ID) is defined by impaired intellectual functions, such as reasoning, abstract thinking, and learning from experience. ID is confirmed by intelligence testing (IQ ,70), combined with assessments of mental abilities and adaptive functioning and evidence that the observed limitations were manifested during the developmental period (DSM-V).3 A recent meta-analysis showed that patients with genetically determined ID display significant improvements in the placebo arm in randomized controlled trials, both on subjective and objective outcomes.4 Yet few studies included a no-treatment control group, and the placebo response (improvement in the placebo arm) could thus not be separated from a true placebo effect, which controls for natural history, being in a trial, and regression to the mean. In From the Department of Clinical Neuroscience (K.B.J., M.P., A.R.), Karolinska Institute, Sweden; Program in Placebo Studies (I.K., T.J.K.), BIDMC, Harvard Medical School; Department of Psychiatry (K.Y., R.L.G.), Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston; Institut des Sciences Cognitives (V.d.P., A.C.), Bron; Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (V.d.P., A.C.); Centre de Référence Déficiences Intellectuelles de Causes Rares (V.d.P., A.C.), Hôpital Femmes Mères Enfants, Hospices Civils de Lyon; and EPICIME-CIC1407/INSERM (A.C.), Bron, France. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article. The Article Processing Charge was funded by Karolinska Institutet. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND), which permits downloading and sharing the work provided it is properly cited. The work cannot be changed in any way or used commercially without permission from the journal. Copyright © 2017 The Author(s). Published by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. on behalf of the American Academy of Neurology order to determine the placebo effect, which rests on treatment expectations,2 studies in other patient populations have developed a method where drug responses in placebocontrolled trials are compared with those in open-label trials, as they represent 50% vs 100% certainty of receiving active drug.5–7 We assessed the placebo component in ID clinical trials by performing a meta-analysis comparing drug responses in open-label vs placebo-controlled trials. Studies were restricted to genetically determined ID, and the statistical analyses were matched for drug type, so that placebo-controlled and openlabel trials compared the same drugs. METHODS Data. We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines for reporting meta-analysis results, and data were obtained from MEDLINE/PubMed and PsycINFO, based on searches for all randomized, placebo-controlled (or open-label), pharmacologic clinical trials in patients with genetically determined ID due to fragile X, Down, Prader-Willi, or Williams syndrome, published before June 2015. Study selection. The inclusion criteria for this meta-analysis were open-label (or randomized placebo-controlled) trials in patients with fragile X, Down, Prader-Willi, or Williams syndrome, of any age, from any country, reported in the English language. In line with our previous ID meta-analysis,4 studies were excluded if (1) outcomes did not evaluate cognitive–developmental functions or (2) the treatment targeted a peripheral comorbidity rather than core symptoms of ID. Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials guidelines were employed to ensure adequate quality of the studies included in this meta-analysis (figure 1). Data extraction and quality assessment. Exclusion criteria were (1) studies that failed to report an inference test or enough information to compute an effect size, (2) studies with fewer than 5 patients, (3) studies that did not provide separate reports for drug/placebo (applicable for placebo-controlled trials), (4) not a randomized trial (applicable for placebo-controlled trials), (5) only treatment results from healthy controls reported. Data extraction was performed independently by 2 reviewers. Discrepancies were adjusted in reviewer meetings and confirmed with a third reviewer. We checked for overlap in participants between studies from the same authors. Based on the patient description including mean age, age range, and date of inclusion, it was possible to rule out the overlap in participants between studies by Erickson, Heller, and Kishnani. More precisely, the mean age and age range did not overlap, especially considering that in all these studies from the same authors, the most recent ones were performed in younger children. In the studies by Berry-Kravis, the majority of patients were not included in both studies. There was some doubt in less than 15% of the patients. It was not possible to rule out some overlap in participants in the studies by Torrioli and Prasher. Statistical analysis. Treatment response was defined as the difference in outcome measures from pre to post treatment within each treatment arm. Calculation of effect size was performed by means of bias-corrected standardized mean differences Figure 1 Flow chart for the open-label drug trials included in the meta-analysis Neurology 88 May 16, 2017 1913 Table 1 Open-label and placebo-controlled studies Study 18a 19a 20a 21a 22a 23 24 25 26 27a 28a 29a 30a 31a 32a 33 34a 35a 36a 37a 38a 39a 40a e-1 e-2 e-3a e-4a e-5a e-6a e-7a e-8a e-9 e-10 e-11 e-12 e-13 e-14 e-15 e-16 Year 2003 2010 2003 2005 2007 2004 2010 2008 2000 2013 2005 2008 2012 2014 2009 2014 2010 2003 2009 2010 2011 2002 2013 2006 1990 2012 2003 2008 2012 2012 2013 2005 2010 2008 1986 1983 1980 1984 1983 Diagnosis DS DS DS DS DS PWS FXS DS PWS FXS PWS PWS PWS PWS FXS FXS FXS DS DS DS DS DS FXS FXS PWS FXS PWS FX DS DS FXS DS DS DS FXS DS DS DS DS No. of patients 6 10 6 17 7 8 8 23 7 12 18 15 48 35 6 16 19 9 56 62 11 14 10 24 15 56 12 24 18 72 50 90 43 106 25 10 69 28 24 Age, y, mean 29 16 51 53 10 28 11 8 19 12 26 2 7 15 18 18 18 30 24 13 47 53 10 31 14 16 9 9 23 52 9 0 1 0 16 11 0 11 12 Male, % 83 80 b b 14 44 100 87 71 83 53 47 42 34 b 88 90 58 63 52 0 50 100 78 47 87 50 100 37 57 85 55 53 57 100 50 56 71 66 Study design Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Open-label Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Placebo-controlled Trial duration, wk 24 20 80 24 16 8 26 14 37 10 52 52 208 12 35 12 8 12 12 10 24 24 12 4 6 4 26 52 16 52 13 104 52 78 26 35 156 35 17 Drug AChEi AChEi AChEi AChEi AChEi Antiepileptic Antiepileptic Antipsychotic Antipsychotic GABA agonist GH GH GH N-acetylcysteine NMDA antagonist Statin Tetracycline AChEi AChEi AChEi AChEi AChEi AChEi Ampakine Anorexic GABA agonist GH L-acetylcarnitine NMDA antagonist NMDA antagonist Tetracycline Thyroxine Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Vitaminsc Abbreviations: AChEi 5 acetylcholinesterase inhibitor; DS 5 Down syndrome; FXS 5 fragile X syndrome; GABA 5 g-aminobutyric acid; GH 5 growth hormone; PWS 5 Prader-Willi syndrome. N-acetylcysteine/L-acetylcarnitine were combined into one category called LAC/NAC. References e-1–e-16 are available at Neurology.org. a Studies that were included in the matched comparison between drug effects in open-label (k 5 12) vs placebo-controlled (k 5 12) studies (drug types represented in both open-label and placebo-controlled studies). b No information given. c Studies using folic acid alone or a combination of vitamins, minerals, or antioxidants. 1914 Neurology 88 May 16, 2017 (Hedges g) using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis version 3.0 (meta-analysis.com). In order to adjust for heterogeneity in the included data, all analyses were performed using random-effects instead of fixed-effects models. In studies with multiple outcome measures, a combined within-study outcome was computed as a synthetic effect size (with a variance that takes account of the correlation among the different outcomes). This approach corrects for the problem of more weight being assigned to studies with more outcomes.8 For a complete list of all outcomes, listed per study, see table e-1 at Neurology.org. An overall meta-analysis was performed to assess the effect size of pre-post treatment within the open-label trials (for a corresponding meta-analysis of the placebo-controlled studies, see our previous report4). In order to compare the effect size of open-label and placebo-controlled trials, studies were matched on drug category. The only criteria for matching studies was that a drug category must be represented in both open-label and placebo-controlled trials in order to be included. Hence, the principle was to include all studies from each eligible drug category, and we did not exclude any studies based on other criteria. A wide range of treatments were represented (including vitamins, g-aminobutyric acid agonists, and statins); for a full list, see table e-2. Six different drug categories were represented both in open-label and placebo-controlled studies and were thus used for the comparison between open-label and placebocontrolled studies. Twelve open-label studies and 12 placebocontrolled studies were included in the matched comparison (even if there was no requirement to end up with the same number of studies) (table 1). The overall effect was assessed using a univariate analysis of variance, using trial duration (weeks) as covariate, and Sidak correction for multiple comparisons was applied to the pairwise comparisons. RESULTS Overall meta-analysis: open-label. Seventeen open-label trials met the inclusion criteria, comprising a total of 261 patients. The mean duration of studies was 38 weeks (SD 48, range 8–208 weeks). Sixty-five percent of the participants were male, and the mean age was 23.6 years (SD 17.5, range 2–53) (table 1). Three of the 17 open-label trials, representing 29 (11.1%) participants, did not report participant sex distribution. We initially identified 13 open-label studies with Williams syndrome but none of them was eligible: fewer than 5 patients in the trial (12 cases) and not evaluating cognitive developmental functions (1 case). The pre-post analysis of outcomes in the openlabel trials showed a significant treatment response (g 5 0.602, SE 5 0.116, p , 0.001; figure 2). In line with our previous reports of ID placebo responses and trial duration,4 we found no significant correlation between study effect size and length of openlabel clinical trials (r 5 0.066, p 5 0.802), indicating that long and short trials had comparable treatment results. Overall meta-analysis: Placebo-controlled. Twenty-two placebo-controlled studies met the inclusion criteria, comprising 828 patients in the drug arm and 721 in the placebo arm. The mean duration of studies was 35 weeks (SD 37, range 4–156 weeks). Sixty-two percent of the participants were male, and the mean age in the drug arm was 17.8 years (SD 16, range 0– 53) and 17.1 years (SD 15.6, range 0–55) in the placebo arm. Figure 2 Forest plot of treatment responses in open-label intellectual disability (ID) drug trials A significant improvement from pre to post treatment was seen across all studies (p , 0.001). If studies included more than one outcome measure they were combined into one value. A random-effects model was used to calculate significance. Neurology 88 May 16, 2017 1915 1916 As previously reported by our group,4 a pre-post analysis of outcomes in placebo-controlled trials (k 5 22) showed significant treatment responses, both in the drug arm (g 5 0.678, SE 5 0.171, p , 0.001) and the placebo arm (g 5 0.468, SE 5 0.150, p , 0.005).4 Open-label vs placebo-controlled trials. The effect of trial type on treatment outcomes was determined by comparing effect sizes between drug-matched openlabel (k 5 12) and placebo-controlled (k 5 12) trials (table 1), comprising a total of 199 patients (openlabel) and 394 patients (placebo-controlled). The effect of trial type was statistically significant (F2,32 5 5.7, p 5 0.008, h2 5 0.263), and revealed higher effect sizes in studies with 100% likelihood of getting active drug (open-label, mean g 5 0.65, SD 5 0.41) compared to the drug arm (p 5 0.043) (mean g 5 0.31, SD 5 0.38) and placebo arm (p 5 0.009) (mean g 5 0.21, SD 5 0.34) in placebo-controlled studies (figure 3). The study effect sizes were not affected by trial duration in any of the 3 groups: openlabel drug (r 5 0.34, p 5 0.287), placebo-controlled drug (r 5 0.15, p 5 0.645), or placebo-controlled placebo (r 5 0.243, p 5 0.447), indicating that ID treatment responses were unaffected by differences in trial length. Figure 3 Drug response from pre to post treatment in drug-matched open-label and placebo-controlled trials There was a significant difference between study effect sizes between drug-matched open-label (k 5 12), placebo-controlled drug (k 5 12), and placebo-controlled placebo (k 5 12) treatment groups. The overall effect was assessed using a univariate analysis of variance and Sidak correction for multiple comparisons was applied to the pairwise comparisons. *Significant at p , 0.01; **significant at p , 0.05. DISCUSSION Our comparison between drug effects in open-label and placebo-controlled studies validate that treatment expectations play a role in treatment of ID, in spite of severe cognitive deficits. In open-label studies, patients (as well as their doctors and families) are assured that the allocated treatment contains an active drug, and thus the contribution of positive treatment expectations is likely to be high. In placebo-controlled trials, however, there is generally a 50% chance of receiving an active drug. The certainty of getting active medication in placebocontrolled trials is thus lower, and may provide less positive expectations of improvement. The effect of certainty in drug trials is previously reported among individuals with intact cognitive functions,5–7,9 and evidence suggests that certainty of receiving the real drug maximizes patients’ treatment expectations, leading to higher treatment responses.10 Here we demonstrate, for the first time, that patients with severe cognitive deficits are susceptible to the different treatment contexts of open-label and placebocontrolled drug trials, similar to the effects seen in non-ID patients.6,7Open-label drug trials in ID patients are therefore biased by the certainty of receiving genuine medication, and should not be used to study pharmacologic effects in ID, as the results will be misleading. Novel therapies for ID that target the underlying pathophysiology are currently being developed, for example for Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome.11 New promising results can lead to a general increase of treatment expectancy among patients’ families, which may enhance the bias (as seen in secretin trials for children with autism12,13) if treatments are not tested in properly blinded placebocontrolled designs. Even if open-label drug trials will not be able to separate the pharmacologic effect from the placebo component of a given treatment, openlabel trials may be used for the purpose of assessing side effects. Nonetheless, research in non-ID patients shows that side effects are also shaped by expectations though so-called nocebo effects,14 and it is important to recognize the possibility that information about side effects (written/oral) may affect the occurrence of adverse effects also in ID patients. In response to the prevalent notion that ID patients are less susceptible to treatment expectancies, we previously performed a meta-analysis where we were able to demonstrate that patients with genetically determined ID display placebo responses in placebo-controlled clinical trials.4 Yet our previous meta-analysis did not compare placebo responses to a no-treatment control, and we could therefore not decidedly link the pre to post improvements to placebo mechanisms (even if natural remission of genetically determined ID would be unlikely). In the present analysis, we employed a balanced design that Neurology 88 May 16, 2017 allowed for interpretation of the role of treatment expectations between open-label and placebocontrolled drug trials. Patients with ID thereby demonstrated genuine placebo effects, which are not explained by spontaneous remission or regression to the mean. There are several possible mechanisms involved in shaping treatment expectations and placebo effects, including implicit and explicit learning.15 In addition to verbal information about the benefits of a drug (requiring cognitive learning abilities), treatment cues may be implicitly embedded in the patient–clinician interaction and clinical environment.16 In patients with ID, it is likely that the implicit social influence of placebo by proxy17 plays a role, which describes the indirect influence of treatment expectations among clinicians and close relatives, and has been mentioned apropos of placebo effects in young children. In brief, placebo by proxy refers to an alteration in the patient’s psychosocial context that may promote placebo effects in a way that is not strictly a learning phenomenon, yet it may have the same profound influence on patients’ response, for example through caregiver’s altered behavior towards the patient. Even if we studied the effects of active drugs (and not placebos) in this study, the placebo by proxy phenomenon is highly relevant for understanding the placebo component of genuine treatment outcomes. Here, the effect may be described as an expectancy by proxy effect on clinical outcomes, reflecting the expectations of surrounding parents, caretakers, and clinicians. Limitations to our study include the inability to control for the possibility that treatment responses rated subjectively by parents, teachers, or caregivers were biased by raters’ knowledge of trial type, despite no or little effect on patients’ actual behavior. We have previously been able to reject the notion that subjectively rated outcomes have larger placebo effects compared to objectively measured outcomes in ID patients,4 and predict that the same is true in the present study. In our previous study, we found that the relatively small number of studies (k 5 22) led to differences in effect size if outliers were removed. We also demonstrated significant differences in treatment responses based on which drug was used in the trial, both in the drug arm and the placebo arm.4 In order to address drug heterogeneity in the present study, we used a drug-matched approach when analyzing the effect of trial type. Yet this reduced the sample size and led to apparent differences in effect size for the overall vs subgroup data. Another limitation is the restriction to include studies that measure treatment effects on core ID symptoms. In the future, it would be valuable to assess the placebo component of treatment for somatic symptoms in patients with ID. AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Conceived and designed the study: A.C., I.K., T.J.K., K.B.J. Performed the data collection: A.C., K.Y., M.P., K.B.J. Analyzed the data: A.C., K.Y., M.P., K.B.J. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: A.C., I.K., T.J.K., K.B.J. Wrote the paper: A.C., K.Y., M.P., A.R., I.K., R.L.G., V.d.P., T.J.K., K.B.J. Interpreted the data: A.C., A.R., I.K., V.d.P., T.J.K., K.B.J. STUDY FUNDING T.J.K. is supported by NCCIH/NIH grant 2K24 AT004095. DISCLOSURE The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Received June 19, 2016. Accepted in final form February 28, 2017. REFERENCES 1. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:679–684. 2. Finniss D, Kaptchuk T, Miller F, Benedetti F. Placebo effects: biological, clinical and ethical advances. Lancet 2010;375:686–695. 3. American Psychiatric Association. Intellectual Disability, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. 4. Curie A, Yang K, Kirsch I, et al. Placebo responses in genetically determined intellectual disability: a meta-analysis. PLoS One 2015;10:e0133316. 5. Papakostas GI, Fava M. Does the probability of receiving placebo influence clinical trial outcome? A meta-regression of double-blind, randomized clinical trials in MDD. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;19:34–40. 6. Rochon PA, Binns MA, Litner JA, et al. Are randomized control trial outcomes influenced by the inclusion of a placebo group? A systematic review of nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drug trials for arthritis treatment. J Clin Epidemiol 1999;52:113–122. 7. Rutherford BR, Pott E, Tandler JM, Wall MM, Roose SP, Lieberman JA. Placebo response in antipsychotic clinical trials: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2014;71: 1409–1421. 8. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2009. 9. Kaptchuk TJ. The double-blind, randomized, placebocontrolled trial: gold standard or golden calf? J Clin Epidemiol 2001;54:541–549. 10. Lidstone S, Schulzer M, Dinelle K, et al. Effects of expectation on placebo-induced dopamine release in Parkinson disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67:857–865. 11. Picker JD, Walsh CA. New innovations: therapeutic opportunities for intellectual disabilities. Ann Neurol 2013;74:382–390. 12. Sandler AD, Bodfish JW. Placebo effects in autism: lessons from secretin. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2000;21:347–350. 13. Williams K, Wray JA, Wheeler DM. Intravenous secretin for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2012:CD003495. 14. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA 2012; 307:567–568. 15. Montgomery GH, Kirsch I. Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain 1997;72:107–113. Neurology 88 May 16, 2017 1917 16. Jensen KB, Kaptchuk TJ, Kirsch I, et al. Nonconscious activation of placebo and nocebo pain responses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:15959–15964. 17. Grelotti DJ, Kaptchuk TJ. Placebo by proxy. BMJ 2011; 343:d4345. 18. Heller JH, Spiridigliozzi GA, Sullivan JA, Doraiswamy PM, Krishnan RR, Kishnani PS. Donepezil for the treatment of language deficits in adults with Down syndrome: a preliminary 24-week open trial. Am J Med Genet A 2003; 116A:111–116. 19. Heller JH, Spiridigliozzi GA, Crissman BG, McKillop JA, Yamamoto H, Kishnani PS. Safety and efficacy of rivastigmine in adolescents with Down syndrome: longterm follow-up. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2010; 20:517–520. 20. Prasher VP, Adams C, Holder R; Down Syndrome Research Group. Long term safety and efficacy of donepezil in the treatment of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down syndrome: open label study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003;18:549–551. 21. Prasher VP, Fung N, Adams C. Rivastigmine in the treatment of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down syndrome. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005;20: 496–497. 22. Spiridigliozzi GA, Heller JH, Crissman BG, et al. Preliminary study of the safety and efficacy of donepezil hydrochloride in children with Down syndrome: a clinical report series. Am J Med Genet A 2007;143A:1408–1413. 23. Shapira NA, Lessig MC, Lewis MH, Goodman WK, Driscoll DJ. Effects of topiramate in adults with PraderWilli syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 2004;109:301–309. 24. Torrioli M, Vernacotola S, Setini C, et al. Treatment with valproic acid ameliorates ADHD symptoms in fragile X syndrome boys. Am J Med Genet A 2010;152A: 1420–1427. 25. Capone GT, Goyal P, Grados M, Smith B, Kammann H. Risperidone use in children with Down syndrome, severe intellectual disability, and comorbid autistic spectrum disorders: a naturalistic study. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2008;29: 106–116. 26. Durst R, Rubin-Jabotinsky K, Raskin S, Katz G, Zislin J. Risperidone in treating behavioural disturbances of PraderWilli syndrome. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;102:461–465. 27. Erickson CA, Wink LK, Ray B, et al. Impact of acamprosate on behavior and brain-derived neurotrophic factor: an open-label study in youth with fragile X syndrome. Psychopharmacology 2013;228:75–84. 28. Hoybye C, Thoren M, Bohm B. Cognitive, emotional, physical and social effects of growth hormone treatment in adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 2005;49:245–252. 29. Festen DA, Wevers M, Lindgren AC, et al. Mental and motor development before and during growth hormone treatment in infants and toddlers with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clin Endocrinol 2008;68:919–925. 30. Siemensma EP, Tummers-de Lind van Wijngaarden RF, Festen DA, et al. Beneficial effects of growth hormone treatment on cognition in children with Prader-Willi syndrome: a randomized controlled trial and longitudinal study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2012;97:2307–2314. 31. Miller JL, Angulo M. An open-label pilot study of Nacetylcysteine for skin-picking in Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J Med Genet A 2014;164A:421–424. 32. Erickson CA, Mullett JE, McDougle CJ. Open-label memantine in fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 2009;39:1629–1635. 33. Çaku A, Pellerin D, Bouvier P, Riou E, Corbin F. Effect of lovastatin on behavior in children and adults with fragile X syndrome: an open-label study. Am J Med Genet A 2014; 164A:2834–2842. 34. Paribello C, Tao L, Folino A, et al. Open-label add-on treatment trial of minocycline in fragile X syndrome. BMC Neurol 2010;10:91. 35. Johnson N, Fahey C, Chicoine B, Chong G, Gitelman D. Effects of donepezil on cognitive functioning in Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 2003;108:367–372. 36. Kishnani PS, Sommer BR, Handen BL, et al. The efficacy, safety, and tolerability of donepezil for the treatment of young adults with Down syndrome. Am J Med Genet A 2009;149A:1641–1654. 37. Kishnani PS, Heller JH, Spiridigliozzi GA, et al. Donepezil for treatment of cognitive dysfunction in children with Down syndrome aged 10–17. Am J Med Genet A 2010; 152A:3028–3035. 38. Kondoh T, Kanno A, Itoh H, et al. Donepezil significantly improves abilities in daily lives of female Down syndrome patients with severe cognitive impairment: a 24-week randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Int J Psychiatry Med 2011;41:71–89. 39. Prasher VP, Huxley A, Haque MS; Down Syndrome Ageing Study Group. A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Down syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease: pilot study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;17:270–278. 40. Sahu JK, Gulati S, Sapra S, et al. Effectiveness and safety of donepezil in boys with fragile x syndrome: a doubleblind, randomized, controlled pilot study. J Child Neurol 2013;28:570–575. 1918 Neurology 88 May 16, 2017