Hindawi Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Volume 2018, Article ID 8128147, 11 pages https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/8128147 Research Article A Double-Blind Study on Acupuncture Sensations with Japanese Style of Acupuncture: Comparison between Penetrating and Placebo Needles Masako Nishiwaki ,1 Miho Takayama ,1,2 Hiroyoshi Yajima ,1,2 Morihiro Nasu ,1 Joel Park ,3 Jian Kong ,3 and Nobuari Takakura 1,2 1Department of Acupuncture and Moxibustion, Faculty of Health Sciences, Tokyo Ariake University of Medical and Health Sciences, 2-9-1 Ariake, Koto-ku, Tokyo 135-0063, Japan 2Department of Physiology, Showa University School of Medicine, 1-5-8 Hatanodai, Shinagawa-ku, Tokyo, Japan 3Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA Correspondence should be addressed to Nobuari Takakura; takakura@tau.ac.jp Received 3 October 2017; Revised 2 January 2018; Accepted 14 January 2018; Published 24 April 2018 Academic Editor: Ciara Hughes Copyright © 2018 Masako Nishiwaki et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. To investigate the acupuncture sensations elicited by the Japanese style of acupuncture, penetrating acupuncture and skin-touch placebo needles were randomly administered at various insertion depths (5 and 10 mm for the penetrating needles and 1 and 2 mm for the placebo needles) at LI4 to 50 healthy subjects. Among the 12 acupuncture sensations in the Massachusetts General Hospital Acupuncture Sensation Scale (MASS), “heaviness” was the strongest and most frequently reported sensation with the 10 mm needles, but not with the 5 mm needles. There were no significant differences in number of sensations elicited, MASS index, range of spreading, and intensity of needle pain for 5 mm penetration versus 1 mm skin press and 10 mm penetration versus 2 mm skin press. The MASS index with 2 mm skin-touch needles was significantly larger than that with 1 mm skin-touch and 5 mm penetrating needles. The factor structures in the 12 acupuncture sensations between penetrating and skin-touch needles were different. The acupuncture sensations obtained in this study under satisfactorily performed double-blind (practitioner–patient) conditions suggest that a slight difference in insertion depth and skin press causes significant differences in quantity and quality of acupuncture sensations. 1. Introduction De qi, a characteristic constellation of sensations felt by patients during acupuncture needling, has long been regarded as an important factor related to clinical effects [1]. For the last several decades, much effort has been made to investigate the quantity and quality of acupuncture sensations in detail. Acupuncture sensation questionnaires such as the Acupuncture Sensation Scale (ASS) [2], the Subjective ASS (SASS) [3], the Massachusetts General Hospital ASS (MASS) [1], and the Southampton Needle Sensation Questionnaire (SNSQ) [4] have been developed as tools to objectively scrutinise acupuncture sensations. Using these questionnaires, acupuncture sensations elicited by manual acupuncture, electroacupuncture [3, 5], and various other manipulations [6] have been studied. Also, comparisons of acupuncture sensations among acupoints [2, 3, 7] and between acupunctureexperienced and acupuncture-inexperienced subjects [8, 9] have been made. The Standards for Reporting Interventions in Clinical Trials of Acupuncture currently recommend reporting de qi in clinical studies [10, 11]. Acupuncture sensations are an essential factor in inducing an analgesic effect [3] and improving local blood flow in the skin and muscles [12, 13]. The relationship between acupuncture sensations and analgesic effects has been clearly demonstrated in clinical trials of acupuncture treatment on epicondylalgia [14] and osteoarthritis of the knee [15, 16]. In recent randomised controlled trials, acupuncture sensation 2 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine questionnaires were utilised to examine the relationship between acupuncture sensations and efficacy of acupuncture treatment [3, 17–25]. In some of these studies, acupuncture sensations reported on the MASS with placebo/sham acupuncture needles were analysed [17, 22–25] in order to better understand acupuncture-specific sensations, as well as the mechanism of acupuncture treatment. However, there have been several limitations to these studies. First, the studies were single-blinded, with the practitioner being aware of which needle was being applied. Second, practitioners did not keep the insertion depth, and in the case of placebo needles, skin press, constant throughout the trials. Also, the methods of acupuncture used in most of the above studies were of the Chinese style, limiting their validity in other styles, such as the Japanese style of acupuncture. Almost all of the studies that investigated acupuncture sensations were performed using 0.2–0.38 mm diameter needles [2, 3, 6, 7, 9, 12–15, 19–21], which are relatively large diameter needles not usually used in Japanese acupuncture. Unlike Chinese acupuncture, Japanese acupuncture typically uses thin needles, 0.16–0.18 mm in diameter [26], with a shallow insertion of <5 mm in depth [27] and a tapping-in method to penetrate the skin using a guide tube [28]. Although there have been clinical studies that employed the Japanese style of acupuncture to investigate de qi, patients reported only whether they felt de qi, as well as a dull sensation, and a skin-penetration sensation [29–32]. To the best of our knowledge, no report has used acupuncture sensation questionnaires to analyse acupuncture sensations with the Japanese style of acupuncture. In this study, we investigated acupuncture sensations elicited with needles in the Japanese style of acupuncture. The study was double-blinded (practitioner–patient) and insertion depth and skin press were kept constant. To measure the sensations, we translated the MASS into Japanese (Japanese MASS) and reported criterion-related validity, construct validity, and reliability [33]. At present, MASS is the most plausible candidate for a standard tool since it is the most frequently applied scale in clinical studies in Englishspeaking areas. Further, it has already been translated into Chinese, and this version has been validated for clinical use [34]. The aim of this study was to investigate and compare acupuncture sensations elicited with penetrating needles inserted 5 and 10 mm deep and with nonpenetrating, skin-touch placebo needles pressed 1 and 2 mm deep. 2. Methods 2.1. Study Design and Participants. The study design was a randomised, double-blinded (practitioner–patient), and crossover clinical trial comprising four conditions. Overall, 50 healthy subjects (32 males, 18 females, aged 22.3 ± 7.5 years) who had normal Japanese language ability and no nervous system disorders were recruited. All subjects had acupuncture experience and knowledge about acupuncture, including acupuncture sensations or de qi. One acupuncturist with 12 years of acupuncture treatment experience (male, aged 35 years) applied the treatment. The acupuncture treatment was Figure 1: Takakura needles for double blinding before and after insertion. Penetrating needle inserted 5 or 10 mm deep (left) and skin-touch placebo needle pressed into the skin 1 or 2 mm deep (right). performed in a laboratory at Tokyo Ariake University, Tokyo, Japan. The laboratory was maintained at 24∘C–26∘C. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tokyo Ariake University of Medical and Health Sciences (approval number 198). The study objectives and protocol were explained to each participant using a written form, and all participants provided written consent. 2.2. Interventions. We used penetrating acupuncture needles and skin-touch placebo needles designed for double-blinding (practitioner–patient) [28–31, 35–37] (Figure 1). For each subject, we prepared 5 mm penetrating needles to insert 5 mm from the body surface, 10 mm penetrating needles to insert 10 mm from the body surface, 1 mm skin-touch needles to press into the skin 1 mm with a protruding blunt tip, and 2 mm skin-touch needles to press into the skin 2 mm with a protruding blunt tip. The appearances of the four needles were indistinguishable. The diameter of the needles was 0.18 mm. Based on our previous study using ordinary acupuncture needles [33], the needles were applied at LI4 (large intestine meridian) [38], the most frequently used point in acupuncture research [2, 3, 7–9, 22, 32, 33], on the right or left hand with a rotating technique until the needle handle made contact with the top of the opaque tube, which was followed by the tapping-in method [28]. After needle insertion, the acupuncturist rotated the needle by 180 degrees clockwise and anticlockwise alternately at 1 Hz for 30 s [5, 8, 9, 39, 40] and then removed it immediately. 2.3. Procedure. Fifty sets of four needles (a 5 mm penetrating needle, a 10 mm penetrating needle, a 1 mm skin-touch needle, and a 2 mm skin-touch needle) were sealed in bags for sterilisation, and then they were sterilised. Before applying the needles, the needle tips were set just above the bottom of Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 3 a guide tube for penetrating needles and just above the lower stuffing for skin-touch placebo needles. Before the trial, all 50 subjects and the acupuncturist were informed that penetrating and/or skin-touch needles would be used. The subjects lay on their backs with their arms resting along their sides. The acupuncturist disinfected each subject’s skin between the first and second metacarpal bones on the back of both hands with ethanol. Then he picked a needle randomly from the set of four and applied the needle at LI4. After the first needle, one of the three remaining needles was applied at LI4 on the other hand. In the second trial after a one-week interval, he applied the remaining two needles at LI4 on both hands in the same way as in the first trial. To exclude bias, we randomly assigned subjects to right or left LI4 for the first needling using a web-generated table of random numbers (http://randomization.com). In the second trial, right and left LI4 were applied in the reverse order to the first trial. As a result, the four types of the needles were applied to both hands in random order (chi-square test, = 0.27). Immediately after removal of each needle, subjects completed the following questionnaires: (1) the Japanese MASS, on which subjects rated the intensity of each acupuncture sensation corresponding to the 12 descriptors on a numerical rating scale (NRS) from 0 (none) to 10 (strongest imaginable); if the subjects felt other sensations besides the 12 descriptors, they reported the additional sensation and rated it on the NRS; (2) a human figure to shade the range of spreading of acupuncture sensation to be evaluated as 0 (none), 1 (localized), 2 (digit/wrist), 3 (lower forearm), 4 (upper forearm), and 5 (beyond) according to the MGH Acupuncture Sensation Spreading Scale [1]; and (3) the visual analogue scale (VAS) to evaluate needle pain, on which subjects rated the intensity of pain from 0 (no pain) to 100 (most severe pain, unbearable). To evaluate the degree of subject and practitioner blinding, the subjects and acupuncturist were asked to record whether they thought the needle was “penetrating,” “skintouch,” or “unidentifiable.” They then rated their confidence in their guess on a 100 mm VAS for confidence from 0 (no confidence) to 100 (complete confidence) [28, 30, 36, 37]. The acupuncturist and subjects also reported adverse events during each needling, if any. 2.4. Statistical Analysis. The Friedman test and Dunn’s multiple comparison test among and between groups were conducted for the numbers and intensities of the 12 descriptors in the MASS, MASS indices (the magnitude of overall acupuncture sensations) [1], range of spreading, intensities of needle pain, and subjects’ confidence. Frequencies of the 12 descriptors were compared among and between the four types of needles with Cochran’s test and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. For subjects’ guesses, Kappa coefficients for 5 mm penetrating needles versus 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles and for 10 mm penetrating needles versus 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles were calculated. Further, Bang’s blinding index (BI) for each type of needles was calculated by the numbers of guesses. The Mann–Whitney test was used to identify differences in intensities for the 12 MASS descriptors and degrees of spreading between subjects who guessed a needle they received as “penetrating” and subjects who guessed a needle they received as “skin-touch.” To evaluate factor structures of acupuncture sensations, we performed an exploratory factor analysis according to the Japanese MASS ratings with penetrating and skintouch needles. Furthermore, to estimate which acupuncture sensations in the MASS were related to subjects’ guesses at needle authenticity, we added “penetrating” in subjects’ guesses as a variable and performed an exploratory factor analysis. A principal component extraction method was used in the factor analyses. Factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were extracted and a varimax rotation involving Kaiser normalisation below 25 iterations for convergence was performed. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Version 24 (IBM Japan, Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). 3. Results 3.1. Numbers of Acupuncture Sensations Elicited, MASS Index, Spreading of Acupuncture Sensation, and Intensity of Needle Pain with Each Type of Needles. The upper rows in Table 1 show the numbers of acupuncture sensations elicited with each type of needles, the MASS index, range of spreading, and the intensity of needle pain. The upper rows in Table 2 show the comparisons among the four needles for each index. There were no significant differences in the four indices between 5 mm penetration and 1 mm skin press and between 10 mm penetration and 2 mm skin press. The rates of occurrence and intensities of each of the 12 acupuncture sensations are shown on the left and right in Figure 2, respectively. More than 50% of subjects felt “deep pressure” and “tingling” with all four types of needles. “Heaviness” was statistically characterised by both occurrence frequency and intensity with 10 mm penetration, but not with 5 mm penetration, 1 mm skin press, or 2 mm skin press. The 2 mm skin-touch needles tended to induce greater “tingling,” “sharp pain,” and “aching” compared with the 1 mm needles. 3.2. Relationship between Subjects Guess and Intensity of Acupuncture Sensations. The intensities of “heaviness” and “aching” with penetrating needles guessed as “penetrating” were significantly higher than those with penetrating needles guessed as “skin-touch” (“heaviness”: = 0.001, “aching”: = 0.015). As for skin-touch needles, the intensities of “heaviness,” “aching,” “dull pain,” and “soreness” for needles guessed as “penetrating” were significantly higher than those guessed as “skin-touch” (“heaviness”: = 0.002, “aching”: = 0.015, “dull pain”: = 0.027, and “soreness”: = 0.030). No significant difference in the intensity of “sharp pain” was revealed between correct and incorrect guesses by subjects (penetrating needles: = 0.087, skin-touch needles: = 0.310). 3.3. Factor Analysis. Tables 3 and 4 show the results of factor analysis in the 12 acupuncture sensations with penetrating and skin-touch needles, respectively. Factors 1, 2, and 3 of 4 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Table 1: Indices of acupuncture sensations and patient and practitioner blinding for each type of needle. Penetrating needles Skin-touch needles 5 mm penetration 10 mm penetration 1 mm skin press 2 mm skin press Indices of acupuncture sensations Number of acupuncture sensations elicited 3.8 ± 3.2 5.5 ± 3.3 3.6 ± 2.8 4.9 ± 2.7 MASS index 2.2 (2.6 ± 2.3) 3.6 (4.1 ± 2.6) 1.6 (2.5 ± 2.0) 4.4 (4.4 ± 2.2) Spreading range 0 (0.6 ± 1.1) 2 (1.3 ± 1.2) 0 (0.4 ± 0.9) 0 (0.7 ± 1.2) Intensity of needle pain 1.6 (13.5 ± 21.2) 11.5 (24.7 ± 28.7) 0 (8.5 ± 16.9) 24.5 (31.2 ± 29.8) Blinding assessment Patient blinding Confidence in guess 61.6 (61.3 ± 25.1) 70.7 (65.1 ± 27.8) 71.9 (65.8 ± 26.8) 76.8 (70.2 ± 26.2) Blinding index (95% CI) 0.12 (−0.14 to 0.38) 0.52 (0.29 to 0.75) 0.30 (0.04 to 0.56) −0.08 (−0.35 to 0.19) [Blinding status] [Random guess] [Unblinded] [Unblinded] [Random guess] Practitioner blinding Confidence in guess 5.5 (15.8 ± 24.2) 11.0 (30.8 ± 35.9) 6.9 (6.6 ± 2.8) 10.0 (18.9 ± 27.6) Blinding index (95% CI) 0.02 (−0.16 to 0.20) 0.18 (−0.02 to 0.38) −0.32 (−0.49 to −0.15) −0.14 (−0.32 to 0.04) [Blinding status] [Random guess] [Random guess] [Random guess] [Random guess] Note. MASS index, ranges of spreading, intensity of needle pain, and confidence level in needle guesses on 100 mm VAS are expressed in median (mean ± SD); confidences in subjects’ and practitioner’s guesses were calculated without unidentified needles. Unidentified needles (% of each type of 50 needles) by the subjects were 12% for 5 mm penetration, 4% for 10 mm penetration, 2% for 1 mm skin press, and 4% for 2 mm skin press. Unidentified needles (% of each type of 50 needles) by the practitioner were 58% for 5 mm penetration, 46% for 10 mm penetration, 52% for 1 mm skin press, and 58% for 2 mm skin press. penetrating needles were inferred as “deep dull-heavy sensation,” “no pain sensation,” and “superficial pain sensation.” Factors 1, 2, and 3 for skin-touch needles were inferred as “superficial pain sensation,” “deep dull-heavy sensation,” and “thermal sensation.” Factor 4 of skin-touch needles, which was a prominent difference from penetrating needles, was inferred as “soreness.” To explore the association between MASS sensations and subjects’ guesses at needle authenticity, we added “penetrating” in subjects’ guesses as a variable and performed an exploratory factor analysis. Table 5 shows the results of factor analysis that include “penetrating” in subjects’ guesses as an additional variable in the 12 acupuncture sensations. “Penetrating” in subjects’ guesses was categorised as the third factor with “heaviness,” “aching,” and “dull pain.” 3.4. Blinding Assessments of Needles. The lower rows in Tables 1 and 2 show BIs for subjects’ and practitioner’s guesses, confidence in guesses, and the Kappa coefficients between needles applied (penetrating or skin-touch needles) and subjects’ or practitioner’s guesses (penetrating or skin-touch). For the practitioner, the BIs of each type of needles were close to zero and his confidences were very low, which indicates that his guesses were random. For the subjects, although the blinding statuses of the 10 mm penetrating needles and the 1 mm skin-touch needles were assessed as “unblinded,” there were no significant differences among the four types of needles in their confidences and no significant difference in subjects’ confidence between correct and incorrect guesses for both penetrating ( = 0.155) and skin-touch ( = 0.819) needles, which indicates that the subjects could not identify the nature of the needle. Except for subject blinding for 10 mm penetrating needles versus 1 mm skin-touch needles, the Kappa coefficients indicated that the degree of agreement was poor for both the subjects and the practitioner. 3.5. Adverse Events. Dot haemorrhage occurred in three cases with the 10 mm penetrating needle. There were no critical adverse events. 4. Discussion In this study, we applied double-blind 5 and 10 mm penetrating needles and 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles to investigate acupuncture sensations with the Japanese style of needling using Japanese MASS. We found that “heaviness” was a distinctive feature for 10 mm needle penetration. The total number of sensations elicited, the MASS index, range of spreading, and the intensity of needle pain with the 5 and 10 mm penetrating needles were similar to those with the 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles, respectively. However, the factor structures of acupuncture sensations were different between penetrating and skin-touch placebo needles, implying that acupuncture penetration and insertion depth may be associated with different sensation patterns. Literature suggests that the depth of needle insertion, that is, shallow (minimal/superficial) or deep insertion, affects acupuncture sensation [41–44] and efficacy [45–48]. This indicates that the depth of needle insertion is an influential factor in determining the sensation and efficacy of acupuncture and that insertion depth should be controlled when investigating acupuncture sensations. In this study, we applied Takakura needles for double blinding and for maintaining a fixed insertion/touch depth [35]. The needles for double blinding can blind patients and practitioners to exclude any bias from psychological factors. Previous studies suggested that acupuncture sensations can be elicited by phantom acupuncture or sham laser acupuncture, Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Table 2: Comparisons among the four types of needles for determining indices of acupuncture sensations and patient and practitioner blinding. Comparisons between 4 types of needles Comparisons between penetrating and skin-touch needles 5 mm 5 mm 10 mm 10 mm penetration penetration penetration penetration versus 1 mm versus 2 mm versus 1 mm versus 2 mm skin press skin press skin press skin press Indices of acupuncture sensations Number of acupuncture sensations elicited MASS index Spreading range Intensity of needle pain ∗∗ = 0.001 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 = 1.00 = 1.00 = 1.00 = 1.00 = 0.134 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 = 1.00 ∗ = 0.017 ∗∗ = 0.006 ∗∗ = 0.001 ∗ = 0.012 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 = 1.00 = 1.00 = 0.199 = 1.00 Blinding assessment Patient blinding Confidence in guess = 0.277 — — — — coefficient [Strength of agreement] — 0.22 [Poor] 0.03 [Poor] 0.42 [Moderate] 0.23 [Poor] Practitioner blinding Confidence in guess ∗= 0.040 = 1.00 = 0.701 = 0.096 = 1.00 coefficient [Strength of agreement] — −0.30 [Poor] −0.14 [Poor] −0.17 [Poor] 0.00 [Poor] Note. ∗∗∗ < 0.001, ∗∗ < 0.01, and ∗ < 0.05; confidences in subjects’ and practitioner’s guesses were calculated without unidentified needles. Comparisons between depths 5 mm versus 1 mm versus 2 mm 10 mm penetration skin press ∗ = 0.036 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 ∗ = 0.045 ∗ = 0.036 ∗ = 0.028 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 = 1.00 ∗∗∗ < 0.001 — — = 0.228 — — — = 0.375 — 5 6 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Rate of occurrence (%) 100 80 60 40 20 Descriptors of acupuncture sensations Intensity of sensation (NRS) 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ∗∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗ Heaviness ∗∗ ∗∗∗ ∗ ∗∗ Dull pain ∗∗ ∗ ∗∗ ∗∗ ∗ ∗∗ Aching Soreness ∗∗ ∗∗ Deep pressure ∗ ∗∗ ∗ Tingling Sharp pain robbing ∗ ∗∗ ∗ ∗∗∗ Fullness/ distension Numbness Warmth Cold 5 mm penetration 10 mm penetration 1 mm skin press 2 mm skin press Figure 2: Occurrence frequency and the intensity of acupuncture sensations on the Japanese MASS ( = 50). The top, middle, and bottom lines of the boxes correspond to the 75th, 50th, and 25th percentiles, respectively. The whiskers extend from the 90th percentile to the 10th percentile. ∗ < 0.05, ∗∗ < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ < 0.001. which implies that patient-practitioner interaction may alter acupuncture sensations [49, 50]. Therefore, we believe that the acupuncture sensations obtained in this study were unbiased in terms of technique. “Heaviness” was revealed as a distinctive sensation with 10 mm penetration insertion (Figure 2). The results were similar to those of our previous study using Japanese MASS, in which 10 mm penetration with ordinary needles of 0.18 mm diameter was applied [33]. Literature suggests that “heaviness,” “aching,” “dull pain,” and “soreness” are all regarded as important acupuncture sensations with manual acupuncture [3, 7, 17, 51]. We found that the sensations of “aching,” “dull pain,” and “soreness” from 2 mm skin-touch needles were not different in frequency and/or intensity from Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 7 Table 3: Factor loadings on the Japanese MASS for penetrating needles ( = 100). Variables Factor 123 Heaviness 0.821 Deep pressure 0.752 Aching 0.732 0.522 Soreness 0.711 Dull pain 0.702 Sharp pain 0.592 0.513 Throbbing 0.542 0.489 Warmth 0.786 Fullness/distension 0.722 Numbness 0.653 0.512 Tingling 0.791 Cold 0.510 0.676 Note. Factor loadings over 0.45 are exclusively represented. Cumulative contribution = 0.707. = 100 indicates 50 of each of 5 mm and 10 mm penetrating needles. Table 4: Factor loadings on the Japanese MASS for skin-touch needles ( = 100). Variables Factor 1 2 34 Tingling 0.842 Sharp pain 0.829 Numbness 0.711 Aching 0.698 Fullness/distension 0.482 Dull pain 0.803 Heaviness 0.800 Deep pressure 0.672 Cold 0.648 Throbbing 0.525 0.594 Soreness 0.776 Warmth 0.549 0.617 Note. Factor loadings over 0.45 are exclusively represented. Cumulative contribution = 0.668. = 100 indicates 50 of each of 1 mm and 2 mm skintouch needles. 10 mm penetrating needles (Figure 2). Furthermore, although spreading of acupuncture sensations has been considered as one of the specific characteristics of de qi [18, 52–54], the spreading with 2 mm skin-touch needles was similar to that with 10 mm penetrating needles (Table 1). Also, despite the finding that stimulating the fascia and muscles was reported to elicit “dull,” “heavy,” and “spreading” sensations frequently [43], our results indicated that only “heaviness” was exclusive to deep needle insertion. Similar occurrences of “deep pressure” and “tingling” with the four types of needles meant that these sensations were also not exclusive to 10 mm penetrating needles. With 5 mm penetrating needles, “tingling” and “deep pressure” were as frequently reported as with 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles, and “heaviness” was much less elicited Table 5: Factor loadings of the Japanese MASS including “penetrating” in subjects’ guesses for all needles as an additional variable ( = 189). Variables Factor 1 23 Fullness/distension 0.730 Warmth 0.701 Soreness 0.659 Dull pain 0.604 0.432 Deep pressure 0.571 Tingling 0.861 Sharp pain 0.723 Aching 0.409 0.572 0.479 Numbness 0.544 0.564 Throbbing 0.499 0.520 Cold 0.494 “Penetrating” in subjects’ guesses 0.819 Heaviness 0.555 0.653 Note. Factor loadings over 0.45 are exclusively represented. Cumulative contribution = 0.594. = 189 indicates number of needles excluding those guessed as “unidentifiable.” compared with 10 mm penetration (Figure 2). Despite needle insertion, “heaviness” was not a specific sensation with 5 mm penetration. Acupuncture sensations with 5 mm penetrating needles were similar to those with 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles, which endorses the potential feasibility of blinding patients using these needles. The differences in acupuncture sensation between 10 mm penetration (deep insertion) and 5 mm penetration (shallow insertion) indicate that a slight difference in insertion depth should not be ignored when acupuncture sensations are evaluated. The majority of indications for acupuncture sensations with 10 mm penetration were higher than those with 5 mm penetration in this study, suggesting that acupuncture sensations with shallow insertions tend to be weaker than those with deep insertions [41, 42, 44]. If the needle grasp felt by acupuncturists as de qi is due to mechanical coupling of the needle with connective tissue [55], a needle depth of 10 mm should contact larger connective tissues than that of 5 mm. This may explain why the 5 mm penetration did not elicit “heaviness,” which was exclusive to deep insertion, if the de qi felt by acupuncturists is assumed to be simultaneously felt by patients. The tip of the 2 mm skin-touch needle is estimated to reach the subcutaneous tissues according to an MRI study performed at LI4 [56]. The stimulation of such superficial tissues with a 2 mm skin press may produce strong sensations. The 2 mm skin-touch needles could be a reliable placebo control for 10 mm penetrating needles because of the similarities in number of sensations elicited (except for “heaviness”), MASS index, range of spreading, and intensity of needle pain between these needles. Interestingly, we found that acupuncture sensations elicited with 1 mm skin-touch needles were quite similar to those with 5 mm penetrating needles as determined by 8 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine similarities in number of sensations elicited, MASS index, range of spreading, intensity of needle pain, and the occurrence and intensity of every descriptor of the acupuncture sensations. These results suggest that 1 mm skin-touch needles could be a reliable placebo control for 5 mm penetrating needles. We found that the intensity of acupuncture sensations was entirely different, especially aching, tingling, and sharp pain, between 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles. This may provide insight into some inconsistent results in other studies using other superficial placebo needles. Several results on acupuncture sensations with single-blind skin-touch needles have been reported. For instance, White et al. investigated acupuncture sensations in patients suffering from hip or knee pain between Streitberger placebo needles and penetrating acupuncture needles and concluded that the Streitberger placebo needle is a convincing control based on similarities in intensity for “dull,” “radiating,” “stinging,” and “electric” sensations [20]. Liang et al. reported that pain scores with sham needle applications using the Park device were larger than those with real penetrating needles [40]. In contrast, it was reported that the intensities of “aching” with verum needles, which is considered to be equivalent to pain [17, 22], were higher than those with the Streitberger placebo needles. Further, it was reported that the intensities of acupuncture sensations with single-blind placebo needles were considerably dependent on needling by practitioners [20, 57]. Taken together, these studies call for a better way to control the depth/pressure of the nonpenetrating placebo needle in acupuncture research. Using factor analyses of the 12 acupuncture sensations on the Japanese MASS, the construct validity of MASS was confirmed using double-blinded, controlled-depth needles. Considering that all subjects had experienced acupuncture in addition to the double blinding of this study, an extraction of the three factors latent in the 12 descriptors, including a factor of sensations unrelated to pain such as “warmth,” “numbness,” and “fullness/distension,” was more valid and reliable than the extraction of the two factors revealed in our previous, unblinded study, in which half of the subjects had not experienced acupuncture. Furthermore, this is the first factor analysis of acupuncture sensations with skintouch placebo needles under practitioner blinding. The factor structure of acupuncture sensations with real penetrating needles was different from that with skin-touch placebo needles. For instance, although “soreness,” which was categorised as a “dull-heavy” sensation, was reported to be one of the particular sensations experienced with real penetrating needles [17], the soreness elicited with skin-touch placebo needles in this study was not involved in the “dull-heavy” factor. It is worth noting that patient blinding was almost successfully conducted from the Kappa coefficients and subjects’ confidence. Also, successful blinding for the practitioner was confirmed with 53.5% unidentified needles in addition to the Kappa coefficients and blinding indices. These results indicate that the methods used in this study have removed much of the bias present in previous studies. Consistent with previous observations that “sharp pain” is frequently elicited by acupuncture stimulation in the epidermis, dermis, and shallow subcutaneous tissue [43], “sharp pain” was the most predominant sensation with 2 mm skin-touch needles in this study. In a previous study, for subjects who believed that the phantom acupuncture they received was real acupuncture, the intensities of “dull pain” and “heaviness” were similar to those with real needles, but those of “sharp pain” and “tingling” were less than those with real needles [49]. In the current study, however, no significant difference in the intensity of “sharp pain” was revealed between correct and incorrect guesses by subjects. “Sharp pain” was not a distinguishing cue for the subjects. To be more precise, the decisive cues for subjects for identifying the needles as “penetrating” seemed to be “aching,” “dull pain,” and “heaviness” from the factor analysis, including subjects’ guesses as additional variables. This was the first study to investigate acupuncture sensations with penetrating needles and skin-touch placebo needles while keeping the depth of insertion and skin press constant. The results obtained in this study suggest that a slight difference in the depth of needle insertion and skin press significantly influences the quantity and quality of acupuncture sensations, which indicates a future direction of studies on acupuncture sensations. Double-blind needles [35] must play a significant role in future clinical acupuncture trials. There are several limitations in this study. Only one acupuncturist participated in this study. We thus cannot test interpractitioner credibility in this experiment. Also, we only recruited healthy subjects. Acupuncture sensations in patients are reported to be different from healthy subjects [53, 58]. Future studies are needed to evaluate the sensations of different types of needles in patient populations. 5. Conclusion Potential factors in the 12 acupuncture sensations on the Japanese MASS were different between double-blinded penetrating and skin-touch placebo needles. The strongest and most frequent sensation observed was “heaviness” for 10 mm penetration. The total number of sensations elicited, the MASS index, range of spreading, and the intensity of needle pain with 1 and 2 mm skin-touch needles were similar to those with 5 and 10 mm penetrating needles, respectively. A slight difference in the depth of insertion and skin press caused significant differences in the intensity and quality of acupuncture sensations. Conflicts of Interest Nobuari Takakura and the Educational Foundation of Hanada Gakuen possess a US patent 6575992B1, a Canadian patent CA 2339223, a Korean patent 0478177, a Taiwan patent 150135, a Chinese patent ZL00800894.9 (title: Safe Needle, Placebo Needle, and Needle Set for Double Blind), and two Japanese patents 4061397 (title: Placebo Needle and Needle Set for Double-Blinding) and 4315353 (title: Safe Needle) on the needles described in this manuscript. Nobuari Takakura is Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 9 a salaried employee of the Educational Foundation of Hanada Gakuen. Acknowledgments The authors express their appreciation to all participants of this study. This work was supported by funds from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), KAKENHI (Grant nos. JP15K08938 and JP16K09266) (Japan). Jian Kong was supported by R01AT006364, R01AT008563, R61AT009310, R21AT008707, and P01 AT006663 from NIH/NCCIH. References [1] J. Kong, R. Gollub, T. Huang et al., “Acupuncture De Qi, from qualitative history to quantitative measurement,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 13, no. 10, pp. 1059–1070, 2007. [2] C. A. Vincent, P. H. Richardson, J. J. Black, and C. E. Pither, “The significance of needle placement site in acupuncture,” Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 489–496, 1989. [3] J. Kong, D. T. Fufa, A. J. Gerber et al., “Psychophysical outcomes from a randomized pilot study of manual, electro, and sham acupuncture treatment on experimentally induced thermal pain,” The Journal of Pain, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 55–64, 2005. [4] P. White, F. Bishop, H. Hardy et al., “Southampton needle sensation questionnaire: development and validation of a measure to gauge acupuncture needle sensation,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 373–379, 2008. [5] A. Y. Leung, J. Park, G. Schulteis, J.-R. Duann, and T. Yaksh, “The electrophysiology of De Qi sensations,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 12, no. 8, pp. 743– 750, 2006. [6] A. Benham and M. I. Johnson, “Effect of bidirectional rotation of an acupuncture needle at LI10 on acupuncture needle sensation and experimentally-induced contact heat pain in healthy human volunteers,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 267–272, 2014. [7] K. K. S. Hui, E. E. Nixon, M. G. Vangel et al., “Characterization of the “deqi” response in acupuncture,” BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 7, no. 33, 2007. [8] H. Park, J. Park, and H. Lee, “Does Deqi (needle sensation) exist?” American Journal of Chinese Medicine, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 45–50, 2002. [9] J. Park, H. Park, H. Lee et al., “Deqi sensation between the acupuncture-experienced and the Na¨ıve: a Korean study II,” American Journal of Chinese Medicine, vol. 33, no. 2, pp. 329– 337, 2005. [10] H. MacPherson, A. White, M. Cummings, K. A. Jobst, K. Rose, and R. C. Niemtzow, “Standards for reporting interventions in controlled trials of acupuncture: the STRICTA recommendations,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 85–89, 2002. [11] H. MacPherson, D. G. Altman, R. Hammerschlag et al., “Revised STandards for Reporting Interventions in Clinical Trials of Acupuncture (STRICTA): extending the CONSORT statement,” Journal of Evidence-Based Medicine, vol. 3, no. 3, pp. 140–155, 2010. [12] M. Sandberg, T. Lundeberg, L. Lindberg, and B. Gerdle, “Effects of acupuncture on skin and muscle blood flow in healthy subjects,” European Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 90, no. 1-2, pp. 114–119, 2003. [13] T.-C. Kuo, C.-W. Lin, and F.-M. Ho, “The soreness and numbness effect of acupuncture on skin blood flow,” American Journal of Chinese Medicine, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 117–129, 2004. [14] E. Haker and T. Lundeberg, “Acupuncture treatment in epicondylalgia: a comparative study of two acupuncture techniques,” The Clinical Journal of Pain, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 221–226, 1990. [15] W. Takeda and J. Wessel, “Acupuncture for the treatment of pain of osteoarthritic knees,” Arthritis Care and Research, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 118–122, 1994. [16] C. Witt, B. Brinkhaus, S. Jena et al., “Acupuncture in patients with osteoarthritis of the knee: a randomised trial,” The Lancet, vol. 366, no. 9480, pp. 136–143, 2005. [17] R. B. Spaeth, S. Camhi, J. A. Hashmi et al., “A longitudinal study of the reliability of acupuncture deqi sensations in knee osteoarthritis,” Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2013, Article ID 204259, 12 pages, 2013. [18] G.-X. Shi, Q.-Q. Li, C.-Z. Liu et al., “Effect of acupuncture on Deqi traits and pain intensity in primary dysmenorrhea: analysis of data from a larger randomized controlled trial,” BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 14, article 69, 2014. [19] S. B. Xu, B. Huang, C. Y. Zhang et al., “Effectiveness of strengthened stimulation during acupuncture for the treatment of Bell palsy: a randomized controlled trial,” Canadian Medical Association Journal, vol. 185, no. 6, pp. 473–479, 2013. [20] P. White, G. Lewith, V. Hopwood, and P. Prescott, “The placebo needle, is it a valid and convincing placebo for use in acupuncture trials? a randomised, single-blind, cross-over pilot trial,” Pain, vol. 106, no. 3, pp. 401–409, 2003. [21] P. White, P. Prescott, and G. Lewith, “Does needling sensation (de qi) affect treatment outcome in pain? analysis of data from a larger single-blind, randomised controlled trial,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 120–125, 2010. [22] A. D. Wasan, J. Kong, L.-D. Pham, T. J. Kaptchuk, R. Edwards, and R. L. Gollub, “The impact of placebo, psychopathology, and expectations on the response to acupuncture needling in patients with chronic low back pain,” The Journal of Pain, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 555–563, 2010. [23] J. Kong, T. J. Kaptchuk, G. Polich et al., “Expectancy and treatment interactions: a dissociation between acupuncture analgesia and expectancy evoked placebo analgesia,” NeuroImage, vol. 45, no. 3, pp. 940–949, 2009. [24] J. Kong, T. J. Kaptchuk, G. Polich et al., “An fMRI study on the interaction and dissociation between expectation of pain relief and acupuncture treatment,” NeuroImage, vol. 47, no. 3, pp. 1066–1076, 2009. [25] X. Chen, R. B. Spaeth, S. G. Freeman et al., “The modulation effect of longitudinal acupuncture on resting state functional connectivity in knee osteoarthritis patients,” Molecular Pain, vol. 11, p. 67, 2015. [26] R. N. Schnyer, D. Iuliano, J. Kay, M. Shields, and P. Wayne, “Development of protocols for randomized sham-controlled trials of complex treatment interventions: Japanese acupuncture for endometriosis-related pelvic pain,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 14, no. 5, pp. 515– 522, 2008. 10 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine [27] A. C. Ahn, T. Bennani, R. Freeman, O. Hamdy, and T. J. Kaptchuk, “Two styles of acupuncture for treating painful diabetic neuropathy—a pilot randomised control trial,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 25, no. 1-2, pp. 11–17, 2007. [28] N. Takakura, M. Takayama, A. Kawase, and H. Yajima, “Tapping-in method (skin penetration technique) with a placebo needle for double-blind acupuncture trials,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 308–312, 2013. [29] N. Takakura and H. Yajima, “A double-blind placebo needle for acupuncture research,” BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 7, article 31, 2007. [30] N. Takakura, M. Takayama, A. Kawase, and H. Yajima, “Doubleblind acupuncture needling: does patient reaction reveal needle authenticity?” Medical Acupuncture, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 169–174, 2008. [31] N. Takakura and H. Yajima, “A placebo acupuncture needle with potential for double blinding—a validation study,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 26, no. 4, pp. 224–230, 2008. [32] H. Tsukayama, H. Yamashita, T. Kimura, and K. Otsuki, “Factors that influence the applicability of sham needle in acupuncture trials: two randomized, single-blind, crossover trials with acupuncture-experienced subjects,” The Clinical Journal of Pain, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 346–349, 2006. [33] M. Nishiwaki, M. Takayama, H. Yajima, M. Nasu, J. Kong, and N. Takakura, “The Japanese version of the Massachusetts General Hospital Acupuncture Sensation Scale: a validation study,” Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2017, Article ID 7093967, 7 pages, 2017. [34] D. T. W. Yu and A. Y. M. Jones, “Physiological changes associated with de qi during electroacupuncture to LI4 and LI11: a randomised, placebo-controlled trial,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 31, no. 2, pp. 143–150, 2013. [35] N. Takakura, M. Takayama, and H. Yajima, “The difference of Park and Streitberger single-blind needles from Takakura double-blind needle,” Journal of Integrative Medicine, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 212–214, 2015. [36] N. Takakura, M. Takayama, A. Kawase, and H. Yajima, “Double blinding with a new placebo needle: a validation study on participant blinding,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 203–207, 2011. [37] M. Takayama, H. Yajima, A. Kawase, I. Homma, M. Izumizaki, and N. Takakura, “The potential of double blinding with two placebo acupuncture needles: a randomized controlled pilottrial,” Medicines, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 11–27, 2015. [38] WHO Regional Office for the Western Pacific, WHO standard acupuncture point locations in the Western Pacific Region, p. 35, WPRO Nonserial Publication, 2008. [39] C.-C. Xie, X.-Y. Wen, L. Jiang, M.-J. Xie, and W. B. Fu, “Validity of the “Streitberger” needle in a Chinese population with acupuncture: a randomized, single-blinded, and crossover pilot study,” Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2013, Article ID 251603, 5 pages, 2013. [40] Z.-H. Liang, C.-C. Xie, Z.-P. Li, X.-P. Zhu, A.-P. Lu, and W.-B. Fu, “Deqi sensation in placebo acupuncture: a crossover study on Chinese medicine students,” Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2013, Article ID 620671, 8 pages, 2013. [41] W. Kou, I. Gareus, J. D. Bell et al., “Quantification of DeQi sensation by visual analog scales in healthy humans after immunostimulating acupuncture treatment,” American Journal of Chinese Medicine, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 753–765, 2007. [42] A. Benham, G. Phillips, and M. I. Johnson, “An experimental study on the self-report of acupuncture needle sensation during deep needling with bi-directional rotation,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 28, no. 1, pp. 16–20, 2010. [43] J. J. Park, M. Akazawa, J. Ahn et al., “Acupuncture sensation during ultrasound guided acupuncture needling,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 257–265, 2011. [44] Y.-J. Choi, J.-E. Lee, W.-K. Moon, and S.-H. Cho, “Does the effect of acupuncture depend on needling sensation and manipulation?” Complementary Therapies in Medicine, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 207–214, 2013. [45] F. Ceccherelli, M. T. Rigoni, G. Gagliardi, and L. Ruzzante, “Comparison of superficial and deep acupuncture in the treatment of lumbar myofascial pain: a double-blind randomized controlled study,” The Clinical Journal of Pain, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 149–153, 2002. [46] T. Miyamoto, K. Itoh, H. Ochi et al., “The efficacy of acupuncture treatment on pain and ability with osteoarthritis of the knee: examination of the curative effect by the depth of an acupuncture needle,” Journal of the Japan Society of Acupuncture and Moxibustion, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 384–394, 2009. [47] S. Fujimoto, M. Inoue, M. Nakajima et al., “Difference between therapeutic effects of deep and superficial acupuncture needle insertion for low back pain: a randomized controlled clinical trial,” Journal of the Japan Society of Acupuncture and Moxibustion, vol. 61, no. 3, pp. 208–217, 2011. [48] M. Nakajima, M. Inoue, M. Itoi et al., “Difference in clinical effect between deep and superficial acupuncture needle insertion for neck-shoulder pain: a randomized controlled clinical trial pilot study,” The Journal Japanese Society of Balneology, Climatology and Physical Medicine, vol. 78, pp. 216–227, 2015. [49] J. Lee, V. Napadow, J. Kim et al., “Phantom acupuncture: dissociating somatosensory and cognitive/affective components of acupuncture stimulation with a novel form of placebo acupuncture,” PLoS ONE, vol. 9, no. 8, Article ID e104582, 2014. [50] N. Salih, P. I. Ba¨umler, M. Simang, and D. Irnich, “Deqi sensations without cutaneous sensory input: results of an RCT,” BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 10, article 81, 2010. [51] H. MacPherson and A. Asghar, “Acupuncture needle sensations associated with De Qi: a classification based on experts’ ratings,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 12, no. 7, pp. 633–637, 2006. [52] Y. Nagahama and M. Maruyama, in Studies of Meridians, pp. 81–101, Kyorin-shoin, Tokyo, Japan, 1950. [53] S. K. Lu, B. H. Hu, and Z. F. Zhang, Eds., A Hundred Kinds of Needling Methods, China Medical Science Press, pp. 32–34, Beijing, China, 1988. [54] K. Zhou, J. Fang, X. Wang et al., “Characterization of De Qi with electroacupuncture at acupoints with different properties,” The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 17, no. 11, pp. 1007–1013, 2011. [55] H. M. Langevin, D. L. Churchill, and M. J. Cipolla, “Mechanical signaling through connective tissue: a mechanism for the therapeutic effect of acupuncture,” The FASEB Journal, vol. 15, no. 12, pp. 2275–2282, 2001. [56] Y. M. Wong, “An understanding of anatomy under the LI4 acupuncture point,” Acupuncture in Medicine, vol. 31, no. 3, p. 333, 2013. [57] A. Enblom, M. Hammar, G. Steineck, and S. Bo¨rjeson, “Can individuals identify if needling was performed with Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine an acupuncture needle or a non-penetrating sham needle?” Complementary Therapies in Medicine, vol. 16, no. 5, pp. 288– 294, 2008. [58] L. Bai, F. Cui, Y. Zou, and L. Lao, “Acupuncture De Qi in stable somatosensory stroke patients: relations with effective brain network for motor recovery,” Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2013, Article ID 197238, 9 pages, 2013. 11